Introduction

Long-term cognitive sequelae of cancer in childhood and adolescence often occur. However, around 60% of pediatric cancer survivors have favorable cognitive development.1 Studies on resilience suggest that the individual and the environment can potentially mitigate the effects of cancer and its treatment and therefore might function as protective factors.2,3 Siegwart et al. (2022)3 describe that the presence of protective factors such as cognitive and social resources was associated with cognitive functioning and quality of life (QoL) in pediatric cancer survivors years after termination of cancer treatment. Strong social and cognitive resources could therefore buffer adverse long-term effects of pediatric cancer.3

It is well known that cancer and its treatment is associated with long-term cognitive difficulties.4,5,6 Around 35% of a Swiss sample of pediatric cancer survivors present a cognitive performance of one or more standard deviation below the normative range.7 Deficits in working memory and processing speed are common regardless of the type of cancer and treatment.8 Working memory is the memory system that holds and manipulates information in short-term storage. It is central to nearly all daily tasks, and serves as a mental workspace that helps to continuously store and use information.9 Processing speed enables one to use cognitive processes effectively10 and refers to the speed at which various mental processes can be executed.11 Working memory and processing speed are closely linked to school success and build the foundation for many complex cognitive abilities, such as reading, mental arithmetic, and attention.12 Due to their slow and prolonged development, working memory and processing speed are particularly vulnerable to adverse effects such as cancer and its treatment.13 Consequently, children and adolescents with cancer need to recover from cancer on the one hand and on the other hand need to master the upcoming developmental steps. Because cancer might disrupt brain development and maturation,5,8,14,15,16 cancer and its treatment can interrupt developmental steps in an age-appropriate manner, leading to cognitive, social and behavioral difficulties even years after diagnosis.17,18,19

Findings in adult survivors of pediatric cancer suggest that cognitive problems may be related to psychosocial functions such as, i.e., emotional distress.20 Cognitive functioning is closely linked to social attainment and poor QoL in pediatric cancer survivors. In particular processing speed performance has been associated with unemployment and worse emotional and mental health.20 This highlights the need to not only focus on cognitive but also on psychosocial long-term outcome in patients after pediatric cancer.

Risk factors known to affect cognitive development negatively include among others young age at treatment, female sex, genetics, cancer type and location, chemotherapy dose, field size of radiation, and longer time since treatment.4,5,15,21,22 Greater cognitive difficulties are associated with a larger field size of radiation23 and higher radiation dose24 as well as intrathecal chemotherapy25 and high chemotherapy dose.24 However, a substantial proportion of pediatric patients with brain tumors (ranging from 20 to 50%), exhibit cognitive difficulties even before initiating cancer treatment.26

The balance between protective and risk factors might account for the variance in cognitive outcome.27 Research on protective factors suggests that both the individual (i.e., personal resources such as optimism) and the environment (i.e., social resources, disease education) can potentially mitigate the effects of risk factors such as cancer and its treatment.28 Protective resources become particularly noticeable when an individual faces critical life events. Therefore, protective factors differ from coping mechanisms used to manage everyday stressors.2 Risk and protective factors cannot be completely separated from each other. For instance, low socioeconomic status might be a risk factor for cognitive difficulties, whereas high socioeconomic status might function as a protective factor. However, little is known about how protective and risk factors are associated with the cognitive development after childhood cancer.

We therefore examined and compared the development of patient’s cognitive and psychosocial functions and QoL at the beginning and one year after termination of cancer treatment. In addition, we investigated how possible risk factors relate to cognitive and psychosocial development. As this study investigates retrospective clinical data to gain insight into the long-term development of cognitive functions, we refrain from the formulation of hypothesis. A deeper insight into cognitive development may help clinicians to promote individual therapeutic strategies that strengthen the maturational trajectory of young patients after pediatric cancer.

Methods

We conducted a retrospective clinical record review on pediatric cancer patients treated at the Division of Pediatric Hematology and Oncology, Children’s Hospital in Bern, Switzerland from 2010 to 2020. The data were collected as part of neuropsychological routine examinations. After the introduction of the general consent at the Children’s University Hospital (2016), data were only used if the patients (or legal guardian) had signed (consented to) the general consent or had been informed of the right to object by means of the general consent. If a patient (or legal guardian) had not provided feedback in the form of consent or refusal after 30 days, it can be assumed that the patient (or legal guardian) has not exercised his or her right to object. Patients who rejected the general consent were not included in the study. Local ethics committee Bern approved the study (KEK-Nr. 2021-00342).

Pediatric cancer patients

All children and adolescents who meet the following criteria were included: (a) treated for an oncological disease at the Children’s Hospital Bern between 1st of January 2010 and 31st of December 2020; (b) aged between 4 and 16 years; (c) available neuropsychological examination in the first two weeks of cancer treatment (T1) and one year after termination of cancer treatment (T2). Children and adolescents under 4 years and over 16 years of age were not assessed with neuropsychological tests at our clinic and were therefore excluded. Additionally, patients who did not complete the neuropsychological tests required for the present study across the two time points were excluded. In addition, patients with preexisting cognitive impairments or developmental disorders were excluded. Lastly, patients who participated in the Brainfit study29,30 during time point 1 and 2 were not included in the analyses. Fifty-seven patients were included in the analysis (25 boys, 32 girls; mean age (T1) = 9.44 years, SD = 2.92 years, range = 4.02–14.41 years). Because the data were collected retrospectively through records, there were missing data in some variables, resulting in different sample sizes (processing speed: n = 54; working memory: n = 41; psychosocial functions: n = 31; QoL: n = 21). The considerably smaller sample size in working memory is due to the restricted age range of the normative values of the working memory index, whereby scores are missing in children <6 years. Further, questionnaires to assess psychosocial functions were often not returned. At T1, nonverbal intelligence was assessed in n = 44 patients.

Outcome measures

Age at assessment, sex, central nervous system (CNS) involvement of the tumor, cancer type, and treatment modality were collected from medical records. The standardized neuropsychological examinations performed in clinical practice during the first two weeks of cancer treatment (T1) and one year after termination of the cancer treatment (T2) included comprehensive neuropsychological tests and questionnaires.

At T1 nonverbal intelligence was assessed using the Test of Nonverbal Intelligence, third edition TONI-3; internal consistency: α = 0.89–0.97; test-retest reliability in the range of r = 0.90 (interval: one week).31

Processing speed was evaluated with the subtests symbol coding and symbol search of the German version of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, third edition (HAWIVA-III; reliability r = 0.76–0.90, for validity see test manual)32 for patients aged 4.0 to 5.11 years. The processing speed subtests coding and symbol search of the German version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, fourth edition (HAWIK-IV; reliability r = 0.84–0.90; validity see test manual)33 were applied with patients aged 6.00 to 16.11 years. In the coding task, the patient draws as quickly as possible as many symbols that belong to the corresponding numbers using a key. In the symbol search subtest, the patient marks whether or not certain target symbols appear in a given row of symbols as quickly as possible. Both subtests are paper-pencil tests and are time-restricted to 120 s. In the symbol search test, the number of incorrect identified symbols is subtracted from the total number of symbols. In the coding test, all correct items are included in the total sum score. An index value for processing speed was calculated out of these two subtests according to the manuals for both age groups.

Working memory was evaluated in patients > 6 years using the working memory index value of the HAWIK-IV (reliability r = 0.89–0.93; validity see test manual).33 This index value is composed of two subtests namely digit span and letter-number sequencing. In the digit span subtest, a sequence of numbers was read out by the examiner. The number of digits increased with at least one of two correct enumerations. First, the patient repeats the digits in the same order, second, the patient tries to remember the order in which the digits are presented, but repeat them in reverse order. In the letter-number sequencing subtest, patients hear a sequence of letters and numbers and repeat the sequence by starting with the numbers in ascending order followed by the letters in alphabetical order.

Psychosocial functions were evaluated with the German parent-report version of the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire.34 The SDQ is a screening questionnaire for behavioral problems of children and adolescents aged 4–17 years. It contains five domains: emotional problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity and inattention, peer problems, and prosocial behavior. Each domain consists of five items using a 3‐point Likert scale. For each domain, a sum score was computed (range 0–10), with higher scores indicating worse function, except for prosocial behavior, where higher scores indicate better functioning. Additionally, a total difficulties score was calculated by adding the values from the four scales referring to problems (without the scale for prosocial behavior; range 0–40). As in Mader and collegues,35 we applied German reference36,37 to categorize patients as normal, borderline, and abnormal for each scale. For the German version of the SDQ, the total difficulty score shows an internal reliability of α = 0.77 and the five subscales present a test–retest reliability of r = 0.58–0.67.38

QoL was measured using the parent-report version of the Inventar zur Erfassung der Lebensqualität bei Kindern und Jugendlichen for children and adolescents aged 6–18 years.39 In the ILK, QoL is divided into six different domains. Each of the six domains is covered by one question. The six domains are the following: school, family, social contacts with peers, interests and leisure activities, physical health, and mental health. Answers are given on a 5-point Likert scale (very good, rather good, partly good, rather poor, very poor). An internal reliability of α = 0.55–0.76 and a test–retest reliability of r = 0.60–0.80 (test interval = 2–6 weeks) has been presented.39 Out of the six domains, a QoL score and a problem score were calculated according to the manual. For interpretation, absolute scores were transformed into percentile ranks. The interpretation for the QoL score is as follows: ≤15 = below average QoL; 16–84 = average QoL; ≥85 = above average QoL. The interpretation for the problem score is as follows: ≥75 = possible problems; <75 = no problems. Whenever possible, all questionnaires were filled out before the neuropsychological assessment.

Statistical analysis

IBM SPSS 25.0 was used for statistical analysis (Armonk, New York). For data visualization, Microsoft Excel (2016; https://office.microsoft.com) was used. A p value of < 0.05 was set as significant (two-sided). Cohen’s d was computed to estimate effect size small effect size = 0.1, medium effect size = 0.3, large effect size = 0.5.40 The normality of the data was examined with the Shapiro-Wilk test.

To compare cognitive performance, psychosocial functions, and parent-rated QoL between T1 and T2, one-sample t-tests or Wilcoxon tests were conducted. Change scores (absolute differences scores; absΔ) were calculated for processing speed, working memory, psychosocial functions, and QoL using the formula T2 – T1. We refrain from reporting relative difference as cognitive performance at T1 is a variable of interest and should not be partialled out of the analysis. Based on the change scores (absΔ), patients were divided into a stable or favorable group (stable or increase of standard scores) and a non-favorable group (any decrease of standard scores) for processing speed, working memory, psychosocial functions, and QoL. The relationship between working memory, processing speed, psychosocial, and QoL change scores (absΔ) was examined with a Spearman correlation. The stable or favorable and the non-favorable cognitive development group was compared in respect to nonverbal intelligence at T1 using a two-sample t test.

Multiple linear regression analyses were conducted to examine the relation between various potential risk factors and cognitive as well as psychosocial functions. Separate regression models were applied for each of the dependent variables: working memory, processing speed, and psychosocial functions at T2. Initially, each model included age at T1, sex, CNS involvement, cancer type, and treatment modality as independent variables. However, due to high multi-collinearity between CNS involvement and cancer type, indicated by variance inflation factors (VIF) ranging from 7.14 to 17.69 across the different models for each respective dependent variable, cancer type was removed from the models. Cancer type was initially represented by three dummy variables, which contributed to the complexity of the model. Given our relatively small sample sizes, ranging from 31 to 54 depending on the dependent variable, we opted for a simpler model that includes only CNS involvement, a dichotomous variable, along with the other predictors. Finally, each model included age at T1, sex, CNS involvement, and treatment modality as independent variables, controlling for measures at T1 of the respective dependent variables to account for initial cognitive or psychosocial functions. To address potential issues of heteroskedasticity, we employed heteroskedasticity-consistent standard errors (HC3) across all models. This decision was based on the recommendation by Long and Ervin (2000),41 who suggested that HC3 could be used reliably regardless of the presence or absence of heteroskedasticity, as evidenced by simulations.42 We did not adjust for multiple testing as our analysis were exploratory.43

Results

Demographic and medical data are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic and medical data.

Working memory and processing speed

Mean performance of working memory and processing speed were within the normal range at both time points and did not change significantly between T1 and T2 (see Table 2). Processing speed was below the normative range (<85) at T1 in n = 8 (14.8%) and at T2 in n = 7 (13.0%) of patients. Working memory was below the normative range at T1 as well as T2 in n = 5 (12.2%) of patients.

Table 2 Processing speed, working memory, psychosocial functions, and quality of life at T1, T2, and their change scores.

Around half of the patients showed a stable or favorable cognitive development, defined as cognitive performance remaining stable or increasing between T1 and T2 (PS: 51.9% of patients, n = 28/54; working memory: 41.4% of patients, n = 17/41). In patients with stable or favorable cognitive development, mean group performance increased significantly between T1 and T2 for processing speed and working memory (Table 2). The other half of the patients showed a non-favorable cognitive development, defined as cognitive performance decreasing between T1 and T2 (processing speed: 48.1% of patients, n = 26/54; working memory: 58.5% of patients, n = 24/41). In patients with a non-favorable cognitive development, mean group performance decreased significantly between T1 and T2 for processing speed and working memory (Table 2). For both processing speed and working memory, the stable or favorable and non-favorable groups did not differ in respect to nonverbal intelligence at T1 and risk factors such as age, sex, cancer type, and treatment modality. Furthermore, there was no association between processing speed and working memory change score (absΔ). Cognitive change scores (absΔ) were negatively associated with cognitive performance at T1, with worse cognitive performance at T1 relating to a stronger increase of working memory (r = −0.511, p < 0.001) and processing speed performance (r = −0.679, p < 0.001) and vice versa (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1: Correlation between performance at T1 and absolute cognitive change scores (abs∆).
figure 1

The first figure (on the left side) shows the correlation between the performance at T1 and the absolute change score for processing speed (r = −0.679, p < 0.001). The second figure (on the right side) shows the correlation between the performance at T1 and the absolute change score for working memory (r = −0.511, p < 0.001).

Psychosocial functions and quality of life

Mean psychosocial functions and QoL were within the normal range at both time points. At T1, 3.2 to 12.9% of all patients had borderline and 3.2–6.5% had abnormal scores in the different psychosocial subscales and the total difficulties score of the SDQ. At T2, 3.2–12.9% of patients had borderline and 3.2–6.5% had abnormal scores (ntotal = 31). The mean prosocial behavior significantly improved between T1 and T2 (Z = −2.327, p = 0.020, d = 0.363).

The QoL score was below average in 14.3% of the patients at T1 as well as at T2. The problem score was below average in 47.6% of the patients at T1 and in 33.3% of the patients at T2 (ntotal = 21). The mean QoL score remained stable between T1 and T2 (Z = −0.434, p = 0.664, d = 0.07) whereas the problem score decreased slightly (Z = 1.503, p = 0.133, d = 0.36), indicating a non-significant improvement in QoL. In psychosocial functions, stable or favorable change scores (stable or increased psychosocial functions) in the different domains occurred in 51.6–77.4% of patients (n = 16 to 24/31), whereas non-favorable development (decrease of psychosocial functions) occurred in 22.6–48.4% of patients (n = 7 to 15/31).

In QoL, a stable or favorable QoL change scores (absΔ) occurred in 42.9% (n = 9/21), whereas a non-favorable QoL change was presented in 54.1% of patients (n = 12/21). A stable or favorable change within the problem score (absΔ) occurred in 61.9% (n = 13/21), whereas a non-favorable change emerged in 38.1% (n = 8/21) of all patients.

Correlation between cognitive and psychosocial functions

The processing speed change score was significantly correlated with the prosocial behavior change score (absΔ; r = 0.472, p = 0.010), indicating that an improvement in processing speed is related to an improvement in prosocial behavior. Neither changes in the SDQ total difficulties score, other SDQ subscales, nor the QoL score or problem score were associated with the development of processing speed or working memory between T1 and T2.

Risk factors

Results from the regression analyses are summarized in Table 3. Multiple regressions regarding cognition revealed that age at T1 significantly predicts processing speed at T2 (T = 2.41, p = 0.020) with older age at T1 relating to better processing speed at T2. Regarding psychosocial functions, sex significantly predicts peer relations at T2 (T = 2.10, p = 0.046) meaning that male sex related to greater peer problems at T2. However, involvement of the CNS and treatment modality did not affect changes in cognition or psychosocial functions.

Table 3 Significant results of the multiple linear regression models for cognitive (3a) and psychosocial functions (3b) as dependent variables.

Discussion

The present study examined the development of working memory, processing speed, psychosocial functions and QoL of patients with pediatric cancer between the beginning of cancer treatment and one year after termination of cancer treatment. Further, associations between cognitive development and changes in psychosocial functioning and possible risk factors such as age at assessment, sex, cancer type, and treatment modality, were examined. Our results suggest that patients after pediatric cancer show individual developmental patterns for cognitive and psychosocial functions and QoL - despite test scores within the average range.

In detail, around half of the patients showed a stable or favorable cognitive development (stable or increased cognitive functions) whereas the other half of the patients showed a decrease in cognitive functions between T1 and T2. This result is in accordance with a recent study showing that more than half of the pediatric cancer survivors had age-appropriate cognitive functions in the long-term.7 Our data suggest, that individual factors such as age at diagnosis and sex play a role in the recovery process after cancer and its treatment and result in stable or favorable (restoration, substitution), neutral (no change) or non-favorable (pathological consequences) development. It is therefore of crucial importance to monitor the development of working memory and processing speed individually to support children with a non-favorable development as early and as well as possible.

Interestingly, in our patients, a significantly stronger cognitive increase was observed in patients with low cognitive performance at the beginning of treatment (T1). We cannot rule out the notion of a regression to the mean, whereby patients with worse cognitive performance at diagnosis might normalize their performance throughout rehabilitation. This statistical phenomenon could partially explain the observed cognitive increase in those with initially low performance and the decreases in those with initially high performance (see Fig. 1).

In respect to psychosocial functions, more than half of the patients showed a stable or favorable development, being in accordance with a study that showed that the majority of cancer survivors do not have an increase in psychological symptoms after cancer and its treatment.44 Further, our data suggest that mean prosocial behavior even improves between the beginning of treatment and one year after termination of treatment. A previous study proposes that prosocial behavior can be higher in cancer survivors than in a norm sample.35 This accentuation of prosocial behavior might be explained by post-traumatic growth that comes with increased focus on positive aspects of life.45 The correlation between the increase in prosocial behavior and the increase in processing speed presented in our results is corresponding with a recent study suggesting that in patients with brain tumor, processing speed played an important role in preventing psychological distress.46 While the directionality of a causal relationship is yet to be determined, research in both clinical and nonclinical samples has consistently demonstrated a relationship between executive functioning (i.e., processing speed) and prosocial behavior (see ref. 47 for a review). Executive functions have been implicated as potential moderators, mediators, and outcome measures in interventions to promote social competence.47 Hence, there might be a close link between processing speed and long-term prosocial behavior.

In more than half of the 21 patients with QoL assessments, a non-favorable development of parent-rated QoL was observed. Factors like parental distress and psychosocial family risk have been shown to influence the parent rating of QoL.48 However, at T1 as well as T2, the majority (namely 85.7%) of the parents reported a good QoL of their children. Hence, our data coincide with other studies showing that most pediatric cancer survivors present QoL scores within the normative range.49

A multiple linear regression model revealed that age at T1 – representative of age at diagnosis – significantly predicted processing speed at T2, supporting existing evidence that identifies age at diagnosis as a risk factor for cognitive problems in pediatric cancer patients. Younger children might be particularly vulnerable to the cancer-related long-term sequelae due to the rapid cerebral development occurring in early childhood. Cancer and its treatment partially interrupts these important developmental processes.5,15,50 Additionally, the regression analyses indicated that male sex predicted peer problems at T2. Female sex has traditionally been considered a risk factor for cognitive problems in pediatric cancer patients. However, recent studies have depicted a more nuanced view of sex as a risk factor. While in certain cancer populations female sex is still identified as a risk factor,51,52 other research suggests that cognitive functions in female patients improve more strongly over time compared to male patients.53 The findings on sex as a risk factor for cognitive problems might also apply to psychosocial functions, given their close relationship to cognitive functions.20

The present study has several potential limitations. T1 data was collected within the first two weeks of cancer treatment. This is an assessment in the very early stage after pediatric cancer. Some patients likely felt sick due to disease and intense treatments, and the parents and families suffered from high distress. Both might influence their responses to neuropsychological assessments and questionnaires. Overall, there is a large likelihood for a selection bias: Patients feeling too sick for a neuropsychological assessment shortly after diagnosis were likely missed at T1 and patients feeling well one year after cancer diagnosis were likely missed at T2 (as they did not see a need for an additional neuropsychological follow-up). Some patients did not agree to the general consent and hence their results were not included in the present data, rendering the presentation of data of non-participants impossible. Furthermore, it is a retrospective clinical record review and hence, the assessments were organized in the clinical routine, in a single center by different clinicians including a heterogeneous group of patients with different cancer types. Missing values are due to the fact that not all patients were followed-up in the clinical routine in a standardized manner. Another limitation of this study is that the patient group is skewed towards higher nonverbal intelligence at baseline. Last but not least, due to the relatively small sample size, regression analysis varied in respect to sample sizes and have to be interpreted with caution. All these issues must be considered when reflecting on the generalizability of the results.

Despite these limitations, this retrospective study is valuable as it reflects the clinical reality of our patient sample and reinforces existing findings from the literature. The variability in cognitive development observed in our sample (favorable vs. non-favorable outcome) highlights the importance of individual neuropsychological monitoring and interventions. Furthermore, the close link between psychosocial and cognitive functions underscores the opportunity and need to foster not only cognitive functioning but also psychosocial competence and interactions after pediatric cancer in the long-term.15 Pediatric patients with cancer often experience social isolation during their hospital stays. By integrating cognitive and psychosocial interventions, we can better support our patients in overcoming multifaceted challenges they encounter during and after cancer and its treatment.54 Future studies could focus on a longer observation periods after pediatric cancer, including additional cognitive variables such as, i.e., executive functions or attention skills and recruiting a larger sample size that would enable the thorough investigation of risk and protective factors.

To conclude, pediatric cancer patients display a wide variability in respect to their development of working memory, processing speed, psychosocial functioning, and QoL with about half of the patients showing stable or favorable whereas the other half experiencing non-favorable cognitive development. The observed variability of developmental pathways highlight the importance of early individualized patient monitoring and the supply of interventions strengthening cognition and psychosocial functions right after diagnosis (i.e., cognitive training, strategy training, metacognitive training, psychotherapy, physical training).