Introduction

Ketamine, a non-competitive N-Methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor antagonist considered a non-serotonergic psychedelic compound, has garnered attention for its capacity to induce alterations in the global dynamics of conscious states [1, 2]. It holds promising implications for pharmacological interventions, especially in the treatment of depression and other mood-related disorders [3, 4]. Specifically, it has been linked to experiences of derealization (i.e., feeling detached from surroundings), depersonalization (i.e., feeling detached from self), and altered perception of the body, environment, and time [1, 2]. Various studies employing behavioral analyses and neuroimaging have illustrated shifts in neural patterns during ketamine administration (see reviews [5, 6]). Although ketamine does not directly target serotonergic receptors like classical psychedelics, evidence from neuroimaging and electrophysiological studies suggest common signatures and potential mechanisms of altered states of consciousness [7,8,9]. Both ketamine and serotonergic psychedelics have been linked to modulations of the Default Mode Network (DMN) [10] as well as increases of brain entropy [7, 11]. Many of the brain pattern shifts observed under ketamine and psychedelics entail a decrease in top-down brain organization paired with an enhanced emphasis on the lower hierarchies of sensory information [12,13,14,15]. These observations are consistent with the Relaxed Beliefs Under Psychedelics (REBUS) model proposed by Carhart-Harris and Friston, which suggests that psychedelics alter consciousness by reducing the weight of prior beliefs when processing bottom-up sensory information emerging from the periphery [15]. The reduced top-down control from higher brain hierarchies results in a richer conscious experience as well as sometimes prediction errors manifesting as perceptual illusions or experience of dissociation [12, 15]. During the ketamine experience, this dissociation may be further evidenced through complexity measures of brain activity, as reflected in increased entropy [15,16,17,18].

Emergent innovations in clinical trials emphasize the adoption of portable dry electrodes, addressing a significant gap in the demand for accessible, reliable, and economical biomarkers to monitor drug effects [19, 20]. However, studies exploring the effects of ketamine with low-density electrodes are scarce. An innovative and robust approach to probe into these effects in low-density setups encompasses brain high-order interactions (HOI) [21]. Three salient features made HOI critically relevant [22,23,24,25,26]. First, as opposed to standard event-related potentials (ERP), oscillations, and connectivity metrics, HOI can compute all possible interactions between signals (here, electrodes). Despite the advantages of having high-density arrays (as source localization, increased spatial resolution, interpolation of bad electrodes), the HOI approach becomes computationally challenging in these arrays due to the so-called combinatorial explosion, inducing a selection bias imposed by optimization techniques, such as greedy search [25], random sampling or simulated annealing [27]. In contrast, despite their reduced spatial resolution, low-density arrays allow us to feasibly compute all the possible interactions, thereby maximizing information conveyance—a feature unmatched by any other technique—and avoiding the potential bias introduced by optimization algorithms. Thus, leveraging HOI provides a unique and novel improvement to understanding brain dynamics using low-density arrays, which usually reduce data’s granularity. Second, HOI effectively capture the global dynamics of brain organization [22,23,24,25,26], a critical component of different consciousness states (disorder of consciousness, anesthesia, transitions and conscious access [28,29,30,31,32]), making them ideal for elucidating the effects of pharmacology. Furthermore, the evidence underscores HOI’ superior robustness when compared to traditional connectivity metrics across various modalities, including fMRI and EEG, and across different brain conditions [23,24,25,26]. In brief, when combined with entropy measures and low-density EEG, HOI may constitute a crucial approach for assessing global dynamic changes induced by ketamine and other drugs used in clinical trials settings.

The current study (Fig. 1) investigated changes in HOI during drug-induced altered state of consciousness, adopting a double-blinded cross-over design, enrolling 30 adults (but only 29 were used in this analysis), and administering racemic ketamine via a continuous infusion protocol (Fig. 1A). We juxtapose the effects of ketamine against a saline infusion as a control, leveraging both task-driven (gamified auditory oddball paradigm) and resting-state EEG sampling methodologies [33, 34]. Using a low-density, wireless, dry electrode EEG system [35], participants’ responses to ketamine were captured. Alongside EEG data, the subjective effects of ketamine administration were measured through self-reported and clinician-administered questionnaires. Our analyses focused on HOI assessed with entropy-based measures [36]. We predicted that HOI will yield consistent results across designs (resting state, task) and conditions (ketamine vs saline, and deviant vs. standard stimuli in the task). We expected that effects would mainly be observed in an alpha band which is systematically reported to be altered in ketamine studies [33, 34]. Such effects are anticipated to be more pronounced during resting state than in task [37], given ketamine’s hypothesized role in diminishing top-down control [15]. This reduced inhibitory control over sensory areas could manifest as an increase in redundancy, as sensory signals could now be amplified and propagated to more brain regions. To assess this specific hypothesis of increased redundancy associated with reduction of top-down hierarchical control, we include other measures of global dynamics (see below). Furthermore, during task-driven conditions, particularly a passive and gamified auditory oddball paradigm, the more predictable stimuli (i.e., standard) are expected to yield similar increased effects in contrast to the deviant stimuli, as prediction may be more impacted by the decreased top-down influences. Finally, we assessed whether HOI-associated changes during ketamine administration correlate with subjective consciousness altered states in terms of dissociative states (i.e., derealization scores) [38,39,40]. In brief, this study aims to establish the ability of a portable, low-density dry EEG device to capture the overarching global brain dynamics of ketamine-induced shifts, both in resting and task-engaged states, serving as potential brain signatures of altered states of consciousness.

Fig. 1: Overview of experimental design and data analysis.
figure 1

A Subjects participated in a double-blind crossover design using portable EEG, capturing both resting states and task-based recordings (namely, a gamified oddball paradigm inducing a typical mismatch negativity). In randomized sessions, participants received both ketamine and saline infusions. B Analysis of high-order interactions (HOI) entailed measurements of total correlation (TC) and dual total correlation (DTC), O-information and S-information (see section “Methods”). C Feature selection across the two designs (rest and task) was employed to pinpoint the primary differences between the ketamine and saline conditions.

Methods

EEG data (16 electrodes, Cumulus Neuroscience dry-sensor 16 electrode headset) were recorded during a resting state and a mismatch negativity task under saline or ketamine infusion (Fig. 1A). A pipeline based on multivariate information theory was applied to investigate whether saline could be discriminated against ketamine under both rest and task. The complete set of possible combinations between electrodes at all orders of interactions (from 2 to 16) was assessed (Fig. 1B, C). The EEG used here has been presented previously on reference [41], where a detailed description can be found.

Participants

Study participants (N = 30 males, 25.57 ± 3.74 y) were carefully selected to ensure a consistent and controlled environment for the research. To qualify, individuals had to be males aged between 18 and 55. Women were excluded to avoid any risk of undetected pregnancy and to reduce sample variability. Participants’ health was critically assessed through a physical examination, medical history, vital signs, a 12-lead Electrocardiogram (ECG), and clinical laboratory tests. Participants were excluded if they had a current or past history of psychiatric disorders as per the ICD-10, especially those with a history of drug or alcohol dependence/abuse in the last 6 months. Furthermore, any serious unstable illnesses, including but not limited to hepatic, renal, respiratory, cardiovascular, endocrine, and neurological disorders, led to exclusion. This also encompassed subjects with unresolved seizure causes, conditions that likely alter brain morphology or physiology such as uncontrolled hypertension or diabetes, significant acute illnesses a week prior to the drug administration, notable history of drug or food allergies, and the use of specific medications including antidepressants, anti-psychotics, anxiolytics, and others. Additionally, color-blind individuals were also not considered for this study. Each participant’s commitment to the study’s guidelines and restrictions was crucial, and they had to demonstrate their understanding and willingness to participate.

Ethics for this study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Otto-von-Guericke University Magdeburg, Germany, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and informed consent was obtained from all participants (Approbation code number: 123/18).

Study design

This study was a placebo-controlled, double-blind, randomized, cross-over study designed to investigate the acute and persistent effects of ketamine on EEG and behavioral measures.

Participants were invited to the laboratory on five different occasions to complete repeated measurements: at enrolment/screening (visit 1); on the days of infusion of ketamine or saline placebo (visits 2 and 4); and on the days after infusion (visits 3 and 5). The two infusion days took place four weeks apart following the same study protocol, while timing of ketamine or saline administration was counterbalanced (Fig. 1). Throughout the study, additional task-driven EEG data collection was remotely performed by participants unsupervised in the home, for a week period before and after each infusion session (not analyzed here). All EEG recordings were performed with the portable dry EEG system developed by Cumulus Neuroscience (www.cumulusneuro.com). The analyses presented in this manuscript correspond to the data collected during the ketamine and saline infusions. The whole experiment was performed just once in the same laboratory, so no replication has been made. The power calculation for this study has an 80% chance of detecting a treatment difference at a significance level of 0.05 (two-sided), assuming the true difference between treatments is 0.750 units, assuming a within-patient standard deviation of the response equalt to 1.

Ketamine infusion

During the infusion, participants were seated in a comfortable chair and their legs were elevated on a footrest. Overall mobility was restricted as a cannula was placed in each arm; one for the delivery of ketamine or saline, and the other to draw blood samples. Participants were administered a single IV infusion (of a total volume of 50 ml) of 0.5 mg/kg of racemic ketamine hydrochloride, over 40 mins, or IV saline solution (0.9%) over 40 min. During the infusions, the tablet was mounted on a tablet holder and operated by the researchers as participants were not able to bend their arms at that time.

EEG recordings

EEG data was collected using the wireless 16-channel dry sensor EEG system developed by Cumulus Neuroscience (Cumulus, www.cumulusneuro.com), suitable for use in a variety of supervised and unsupervised settings [35]. The analog front-end is based on the ADS1299 chip-set from Texas Instruments, incorporating high input impedance of 1GΩ, a configurable driven bias function for common-mode rejection, built-in impedance checking, and configurable gain and sampling rates. The left mastoid is used for reference and the right mastoid for driven-bias, with single-use, snap-on electrodes attached to wires extending from the headset. An onboard processor and Bluetooth module transmit 250 Hz EEG data to another device (an Android tablet in this case), transferring it to a cloud server for storage and processing. Flexible Ag/AgCl coated polymer sensors of a comb-design (ANT-Neuro/eemagine GmbH) are used to achieve a stable and dermatologically safe contact to the scalp through the hair. The electronics and sensors are mounted on a flexible neoprene net for comfort and ease of montage. The incorporation of natural landmarks in the headset form factor and the stretchable structure both enable consistent placement by non-experts in line with the 10–20 sensor system.

Resting-state session

Eyes-closed resting state EEG recordings were collected during the first 20 min of the 40 min continuous infusions. We expected ketamine effects to build-up linearly over time as the plasma concentration increased. For this reason and to maximize detection of any effects, we analyzed the later half of the resting state data during which ketamine concentration would be higher. Participants were instructed to rest with their eyes closed and remain still during the recording.

Passive-listening auditory oddball paradigm

During the last 15 min of each infusion, we used an app-based version of the passive auditory oddball task developed by Cumulus— Sonic Scenes—eliciting the Mismatch Negativity (MMN) of infrequent ‘deviant’ stimuli in a train of ‘standard’ stimuli. The task was performed passively – the subject merely needed to remain still while listening to repetitive auditory stimuli. The participant was prompted to put on headphones and adjust the volume until he could clearly hear some sample tones. When ready, the participant asked to press play on a silent movie, which lasted 15 min. Tones played throughout, while the participant watched the film. Eight short films were used in an arbitrary cycled order, each consisting of silent clip montages compiled from stock footage. There were no narratives nor subtitles. Each movie session incorporated 1000 ‘standard’ tones (100 ms; 1000 Hz) and 200 pitch deviants (100 ms; 1200 Hz). The inter-onset interval was of 650 ms fixed as recommended by Duncan et al. [42]. To verify that the sound volume selected by participants for the gamified MMN task was stable throughout the study, we ran a linear regression on the recorded tablet volume against the session number, i.e. the number of times a user has performed the MMN task. Neither initial volume nor final volume - i.e. volume after incorporating any volume changes within the task - demonstrated an increase with session number (See Supplementary Table 3).

Subjective scores of dissociative states

We assessed the Clinician-Administered Dissociative States Scale (CADSS), which measures dissociative states [43], and the 5D-ASC, which assesses self-reported altered states of consciousness [44]. CADSS has 3 subscales (“Amnesia”, “Derealization”, “Depersonalization”), while 5D-ASC entails five dimensions (“Oceanic Boundlessness”, “Dread of Ego Dissolution (DED)”, “Visionary Restructuralization”, “Auditory Alterations”, “Vigilance Reduction“). Both questionnaires were administered in the laboratory 1 h before and 1.5 h after the infusion. When answering after the infusions, participants were asked to answer according to the highest point of dissociative experience during infusion. The scores’ difference between before and after infusion were used to rate the subjective changes both for saline and for ketamine infusion. All questionnaires used validated German-language versions, except for the CADSS questionnaire which was translated by the research team as no validated translation was available at the time the study was conducted.

EEG data preprocessing

In the Cumulus platform, EEG data is automatically uploaded to the cloud and a proprietary processing pipeline is applied, which is designed to quantify signal quality, and selectively aggregate individual trials to deal with the noise and signal variation seen in less controlled dry-sensor recordings. This verifies the integrity of timing information and excludes bad quality signal portions. Corrective procedures are applied for missing and anomalous data, including eye and other characteristic artifacts. After that, EEG signals were pre- processed with filtering from 0.5–40 Hz, epoch extraction, and baseline adjustment. All data were recorded with a left-mastoid reference.

To preserve the same number of channels (16) during the whole analysis, time points from all channels were removed if at least one channel had a flat signal in those points, which was required to explore all the possible high-order interactions. One subject was removed from further resting state analysis because there was <1 s of valid data in the ketamine condition. No differences in the number of selected points were found after the artifact removal (Supplementary Tables 1 and 2) across conditions (ketamine/saline) and recordings (rest/task). Resting state data were bandpass filtered in the canonical EEG frequency bands: δ: 0.5–4 Hz, θ: 4–8 Hz, α: 8–12 Hz, β: 12–30 Hz, γ: 30–40 Hz). Note that the definition of the γ range here has been elsewhere used for EEG [45] and ketamine [46] and should not be confused with the γ range used in intracranial recordings, which reaches much higher frequencies. Given that our goal was not to compute ERPs, we pooled all the valid trials (with at least 125 ms valid signal) corresponding to the deviant and standard tones separately to perform the analysis. In the following analysis, the 5 bands plus broadband (0.5–40 Hz) data was used for the resting state, while for the tasks only broadband data were used to focus only on the evoked rather than in the induced response. EEG data obtained appeared reliable as the standard effects on ketamine vs. saline solution were noticeable in the EEG spectrum (across the alpha peak and typical frequency/power decay, see Fig. S1).

Pairwise and high-order interactions

To assess the hypothesis of ketamine-induced specific effects of redundancy in brain dynamics, different measures of entropy characterizing different properties of brain dynamics were computed. We used tools from multivariate information theory i) to quantify the nonlinear statistical dependencies between all the possible combinations between electrodes and ii) to distinguish the nature of these dependencies in terms of collective constraints (total correlation, TC), shared randomness (dual total correlation, DTC), synergy (O-info<0), redundancy (O-info>0), and overarching correlations (S-info) (see reference [36] for a detailed explanation of these measures).

Consider a system of n random variables denoted by Xn = (X1, …, Xn). The TC, DTC, O-information (O) and S-information (S) are generalizations of the mutual information (MI) [36], which can be respectively expressed as:

$${TC}({X}^{n})=\mathop{\sum }\limits_{i=1}^{n}H({X}_{i})-H({X}_{1},...,{X}_{n})$$
(1)
$${DTC}({X}^{n})=H({X}_{1},...,{X}_{n})-\mathop{\sum }\limits_{i=1}^{n}H({X}_{i}{\rm{|}}{{X}^{n}}_{-i})$$
(2)
$$O({X}^{n})={TC}({X}^{n})-{DTC}({X}^{n})$$
(3)
$$S({X}^{n})={TC}({X}^{n}+{DTC}({X}^{n})$$
(4)

where \(H({X}_{1},...,{X}_{n})\) is the joint Shannon’s entropy of the n variables, \(H({X}_{i})\) the entropy of the i-th region and \(H({X}_{i}|{{X}^{n}}_{-i})\) is the entropy of the i-th region conditioned by the activity of the whole system without it - which is known as “residual entropy,” and is denoted as \({R}_{i}\). Above, X-in stands for the vector of all variables except Xi, i.e., (X1, …, Xi-1, Xi+1, …, Xn). Estimations were performed using the Gaussian copula approximation [25, 47]. As for n = 2 TC = DTC = mutual information, only the TC was computed for each possible pair of electrodes. For the high-order interactions (from 3 to 16) all metrics were computed for each possible combination of electrodes at each order of interaction (Fig. 1B, C). Despite the mutual information is directly related to the Pearson correlation coefficient in the pairwise case, the Gaussian copula approach, by preserving the copula function between the variables, can capture nonlinear high-order dependencies as sinergy [48]. Although pairwise interactions completely define a multivariate Gaussian, entropy computation makes use of the determinant of the matrix, which is a property of the whole matrix rather than of the pairwise interactions (for a notable example of high-order statistical interactions in a full pairwise system see reference [49]).

Broadband (0.5–40 Hz) and filtered EEG signals (δ: 0.5–4 Hz, θ: 4–8 Hz, α: 8–12 Hz, β: 12–30 Hz, γ: 30–40 Hz) were analyzed considering all the possible combinations of electrodes at each order of interaction, here denoted by k (120 interactions for k = 2, 560 for k = 3, 1.820 for k = 4, 4.368 for k = 5, 8.008 for k = 6, 11.440 for k = 7, 12.870 for k = 8, 11.440 for k = 9, 8.008 for k = 10, 4.368 for k = 11, 1.820 for k = 12, 560 for k = 13, 120 for k = 14, 16 for k = 15 and 1 for k = 16). An n-plet represents a particular combination of n electrodes, and the effect of ketamine on it was assessed via the effect sizes.

Effect sizes

To characterize the size of the effect associated with each measurement we used the Cohen’s \(d\) effect size for paired samples [50]:

$$d=\frac{{\mu }_{{ket}}-{\mu }_{{sal}}}{s}$$
(5)

where \({\mu }_{{ket}}\) and \({\mu }_{{sal}}\) are the average measure of the ketamine and saline condition, respectively, and \(s\) is the standard deviation of the difference of means (i.e \({\mu }_{{ket}}-{\mu }_{{sal}}\)). This metric measures a standardized mean difference between paired samples, and its sign indicates the direction of the effect, i.e. if d > 0 means that ketamine increases the measure.

Statistical analyses

For the selected features, a two-sided non-parametric Wilcoxon sign rank test for paired samples with the False Discovery Rate (FDR) correction for multiple comparisons was performed. As in previous work on HOI [22,23,24,25,26], statistical correction was not directly assessed for each HOI given the non-selective data approach including all interactions [51]. Conversely, we used Cohen’s D to report the effect size of HOI [22, 24, 25] as p-values can be artificially inflated. To compute the association between HOI and subjective scores, the change (ketamine - saline) in HOI was (Pearson) correlated with the change in subjective scores, yielding one R2 per n-plet and subjective score. We evaluated the significance of associations by a permutation test followed by post-hoc FDR correction for multiple comparisons. First, we generated a null distribution of R2 values by randomly permuting the correspondence between HOI and subjective scores 1000 times. Only correlations values above the 99.9-th percentile of their respective null distribution were then submitted to FDR correction (p < 0.005).

Results

Ketamine increases redundancy in the alpha band during resting state

Results evidenced both positive and negative effect sizes for all the combinations between filtering bands and metrics. The alpha band showed the largest (metric increase with ketamine) and smallest effect sizes (metric decrease with ketamine) for all the metrics (Fig. 2A and S1). Indeed, only O-info and S-info significantly increased in the alpha band (Fig. 1B, Wilcoxon sign rank test, p < 0.001 after FDR correction). Among the 4 metrics in the alpha band, the largest increase was found for the O-info, which included the P3, FCC3, Fz, and FT8 electrodes (Fig. 2C). The S-info was related to the CPz, FT7, and FT8 electrodes. Despite the other filtering bands also exhibiting a marked tendency both for increases and decreases, no significant effect was found (Fig. S2). These results indicate that the effect of ketamine can be better explained as an increase in the overarching correlations—specifically redundancy—between temporal and parietal recordings in the alpha band during the resting state.

Fig. 2: Ketamine increases redundancy in the resting state alpha band.
figure 2

A Each panel shows the effect sizes of each n-plet (i.e. each possible combination of electrodes from 2 to 16) sorted in decreasing order, with a red horizontal line showing the 0. The red dot denotes the maximum effect size. B The n-plet with the largest effect under saline and ketamine for each subject (gray) and for the average (black). Only O and S-info yielded FDR-corrected Wilcoxon sign rank p-values < 0.001. C EEG layout with the electrodes composing the n-plet in black.

To assess if EEG dynamics revealed through HOI analysis were also reflected by conventional pairwise connectivity analyses, pairwise connectivity was investigated using information theory metrics, specifically mutual information and conditional mutual information. The ability of a machine-learning classifier informed with EEG pairwise functional connectivity to discriminate conscious states was assessed, and connections best explaining the classification model were identified. The classification power of the model was low (AUC < 0.8), and connections contributing most to the classification model did not differ between states of consciousness (Wilcoxon sign rank test, p > 0.1) (Fig. S5). These results underscore the validity of HOI analysis to effectively capture EEG dynamics that are not observable through pairwise analyses.

Ketamine increases redundancy for predictable stimuli

We found both positive and negative effect sizes for all the combinations between metrics and stimuli type (STD, standard; DEV, deviant) for the auditory oddball task (Figs. 3A and S4). We compared the STD and DEV response under saline and under ketamine, without comparing the STD to the DEV tone. The deviant tone showed slightly larger absolute effect sizes. Significant differences were found only for the O-info in the standard tone (Fig. 3B, FDR-corrected Wilcoxon sign rank test, p < 0.005), which included CPz, Cz and FCz electrodes. The results for the rest of the metrics and the deviant tones are shown in Fig. S4. The same set of electrodes was found for both conditions in the tasks. These results indicate that ketamine significantly increases the evoked responses for predictable but not deviant stimuli.

Fig. 3: Redundancy increases in the standard tone for mismatch negativity task.
figure 3

A Effect sizes of each n-plet for the oddball paradigm, sorted in decreasing order, with a red horizontal line showing the 0. The red dot denotes the maximum effect size B The n-plet with the largest effect under saline and ketamine for each subject (gray) and for the average (black). C The electrodes involved in the best feature. Wilcoxon sign rank FDR-corrected p-value < 0.005.

Derealization correlates with changes in resting state theta-band high-order interactions

Finally, we investigated the association between the change in resting state HOI and the change in subjective scores, as measured via the CADSS and the 5D-ASC (see section “Methods”). We found the largest number of significant correlations for the O-info in the theta band and CADSS Derealization score, followed by the alpha band in the ‘Dread of ego dissolution’ item (Fig. 4B). Indeed, the theta band showed the largest association values for all the subjective scores, followed by the alpha band (Fig. S6). The presence of the alpha band is consistent with results of the previous section, where the largest effect sizes were found in the alpha-band.

Fig. 4: Association between subjective scores and HOI.
figure 4

A Average R2 only for the significant (FDR, p < 0.005) associations between the change in each dimension of subjective score (see section “Methods”) and change in HOI. B Same as A, but showing the number of significant HOI in log10 scale to improve visualization.

This finding suggests that changes in the alpha band are not only indicative of the change in the global state of consciousness, but also of more subtler aspects of the experience. In turn, changes in the theta band HOI may be indicative of the level of derealization experienced by the subjects following ketamine infusion, as compared to saline.

Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the neuropharmacological effects of ketamine on consciousness, employing a novel and robust approach using HOI, with portable EEG in a double-blinded cross-over design. This exploration centered around low-density arrays, addressing a current gap in research methodology. Key findings indicate that ketamine administration induced a significant increase in the correlations and redundancy of brain dynamics, particularly evident in the alpha frequency band, consistent with observations across classical EEG studies of ketamine. Results open a new avenue for future studies using portable, low-density recordings captured during pharmacological interventions that maximize the combined information across electrodes.

Our findings bolster the application of HOI for low-density arrays. The shifts in brain dynamics, specifically an increase in redundancy, are most pronounced in alpha (echoing previous reports focused on this band, tasks and rest [52,53,54]). Such alterations, particularly in the resting state, supports the potential of ketamine to decrease top-down control and increase the sensitization to bottom-up signals [1, 2, 12,13,14,15]. This phenomenon is manifested by an upswing in redundancy within brain dynamics, possibly reflecting a reduced top-down inhibition and an amplification of lower hierarchies of sensory information [12,13,14,15]. The increase in redundancy following ketamine administration was larger during resting states than tasks, also suggesting the spontaneous shift of the conscious state towards less controlled states during resting state, as dissociation. Also, the app-based auditory oddball paradigm revealed that the effects on redundancy are more conspicuous for predictable (standard) stimuli than for deviant stimuli, accentuating ketamine’s influence on spontaneous, less controlled cognitive processes [12, 15], as low-level stimuli prediction. Furthermore, a significant correlation emerged between the effects of ketamine on resting-state HOI in the alpha and theta band and the subjective experiences of DED and derealization, respectively, both core facets of ketamine-induced dissociative states. DED refers to a change in the perception of selfhood and subject-object boundaries, while derealization is the feeling of disconnection from the surroundings [12, 15, 16]. These results confirm reported associations between the alpha-band and ´ego-integrity’ [55] as well as derealization measures and theta band [56, 57], particularly observed with ketamine [12]. Although it has been reported that ketamine modulates the gamma band [46, 58], our results show that changes in power spectrum can be dissociated from changes in HOI (we confirmed the gamma modulation in Figure S1), suggesting that these two aspects of brain dynamics could reflect different processes. Our findings support the idea that subjective alterations in consciousness are anchored in changes of brain dynamics and functional organization, and thus can be properly tracked with measures of neural complexity [59].

This study has limitations that open different avenues for further research. Compared to high-density electrodes, low-density dry electrodes offer limited information and have a lower signal-to-noise ratio. However, we attempted to mitigate these shortcomings with several internal controls in our design, including (a) no significant difference in the number of artifacts across conditions; (b) using a robust double-blinded cross-over design, which minimized individual heterogeneity, variance, and the distribution of random effects from any confounding variables across conditions; (c) verification of the expected effects of ketamine on the power spectrum (supplementary data). Indeed, the investigation of the minimal electrode layout that maximizes information retrieval from brain signals can potentially make EEG more accessible for scientific and clinical application but this is out of the scope of our current work. Further, we found a systematic directional consistency of the effects of HOI across various measures. Moreover, since the primary HOI differentiating the ketamine vs. saline conditions did not involve frontal areas, the results reduce the possibility that differences in eye movement across conditions could account for these effects. Despite these measures, our findings require further validation using high-density electrode arrays, though this might entail reducing the number of HOI. Exploring other brain measures like fMRI, where HOI can be assessed, would also be beneficial. While our sample size was modest, it remains comparable to, or even larger than, similar studies in the field (see reviews: [5, 6, 12, 13]). In any case, future studies need to replicate the findings in larger samples. This study only included male participants, so the effects of gender and the potential particular changes observed in women require further investigation. Lastly, our study lacks an exploration of brain dynamics in correlation with plasma measurements of ketamine and potential whole-body effects, such as cardio-dynamics and sensorimotor activity. These aspects offer promising avenues for future research.

Conclusions

Our results suggest that HOI provide a novel approach to maximizing the information obtained from low-density EEG in pharmacological interventions. More specifically, ketamine fosters an increase in redundancy and HOI across measures suggesting changes in the way the brain processes information, leading towards a state of dissociation. Further research is needed to evaluate the potential of HOI to track the potential therapeutic effect of ketamine for psychiatric diseases such as depression. These effects offer a deeper understanding of the neuropharmacological actions of ketamine and underscore the potential of portable EEG devices in charting alterations in consciousness, especially when combined with the HOI in low-density setups. This could lay the foundation for future endeavors aimed at better capturing the subject’s preparedness and the subsequent pharmacological-induced changes in therapeutic settings across neuropsychiatric conditions. Finally, the use of portable EEG to track the effect of psychopharmacological interventions in non-clinical environments could enhance the accessibility to neurological exams especially for patients with reduced mobility.