Introduction

Forming memories is essential for survival, guiding our behaviors based on past experience. The expression of most these memories typically weakens over time as an adaptive process of forgetting [1]. Whereas, in some types of maladaptive memories, such as the drug-cue associated memories which account for the risk of drug relapse [2], their expression is progressively enhanced from recent memory to remote one during prolonged time [3]. A similar phenomenon that cue-induced drug craving was abstinent-dependently increased (known as incubation of cue-induced drug craving) has been widely observed in rodents and humans abused with drugs [4,5,6], further suggesting the influence of remote drug memory enhancement on the internal and external states.

The formation of remote memory depends on system-level of memory consolidation, during which specific brain regions undergo reorganization, with subcortical areas responsible for recent memory and the neocortex involved in permanent storage of remote memory [7]. Previous studies have reported that memory enhancement can be modulated through reconsolidation process [8], but we have a limited understanding of the neural underpinnings about enhancement of remote drug-cue associated memory through system consolidation.

The prelimbic cortex (PrL) has been shown to be important for the consolidation of remote memory. Numerous studies have identified that the PrL can function as a storage hub of remote memory, and this role gradually gets matured during the system consolidation process, evidenced by the specific reactivation by remote but not recent memory expression [9, 10]. Furthermore, this functional maturation of PrL required dense upstream inputs, such as from the basolateral amygdala (BLA) and hippocampus [9,10,11]. Optogenetically silencing projections from the hippocampus or amygdala effectively dampened synaptic plasticity of the prefrontal cortex, attenuating the expression of remote memory [9, 12]. Although these studies identified the contribution of PrL to consolidation of remote memory, whether the enhancement of remote drug-cue associated memory is established through the PrL during system consolidation has far been unknown. Of these broad upstream inputs, the BLA has shown greater potential in modulating drug-cue associated memory [13, 14]. However, it remains to be determined whether and how the BLA input participates in the enhancement of remote drug memory via assisting the PrL.

In this study, we found that the enhancement of remote cocaine memory resulted from memory system consolidation via a stepwise increase in response to cues and mature dendritic spines in the PrL, which depended on the BLA projection during an early period of system consolidation.

Materials and methods

Animals

Male Sprague-Dawley rats (weighing 280–300 g upon arrival) were purchased from Vital River Laboratories. The rats were housed five per cage for 2 weeks before the experiments and then were individually housed after virus delivery or self-administration surgery. All rats were maintained on a reverse 12 h/12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 8:00 AM) with free access to food and water. All animal procedures were performed in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and approved by the Biomedical Ethics Committee for animal use and protection of Peking University. Our estimates of the number of animals that were required for behavioral testing (n ≥ 5 per group), immunofluorescence (n ≥ 3 per group), fiber photometry (n ≥ 7 per group), and spine analysis (n ≥ 5 per group) were based on our past experience in the laboratory and previous research.

Surgery

Self-administration surgery was performed as described previously [15, 16]. The rats were anesthetized with isoflurane (4–5% for induction; 1.5–2% for maintenance). Silastic catheters were inserted in the jugular vein, passed to the midscapular region, and attached to a cannula. To sterilize the catheters and keep them clear, we injected penicillin (North China Pharmaceutical Group Corporation, 80 U/mg, dissolved in 0.2% heparin sodium at 0.25 mg/mL) in the cannula daily until the end of self-administration training.

For calcium imaging, AAV-CaMKIIα-GCaMP6s-WPRE-hGH polyA (AAV2/9, ≥2.00 × 1012 vector genome/mL) was unilaterally injected in the PrL or BLA (300 nL/side).

For the chemogenetic inhibition of PrL and BLA CaMKII neurons, AAV-CaMKIIα-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry-WPRE polyA (AAV2/9, 1.00 × 1013 vector genome/mL) and AAV-CaMKIIα-mCherry-WPRE polyA (AAV2/9, 1.0 × 1013 vector genome/mL) were diluted with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) at a ratio of 1:1 and injected in the PrL (500 nL/side) or BLA (300 nL/side). For chemogenetic inhibition of the BLA→PrL or BLA → IL glutamatergic projection, we injected retroAAV-hSyn-GFP-Cre-WPRE polyA (AAV2/R, 1.0 × 1013 vector genome/ml) in the PrL (400 nL/side) or IL (250 nL/side) and AAV-CaMKIIα-DIO-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry-WPRE-hGH polyA (AAV2/9, 1.00 × 1013 vector genome/mL) in the BLA (300 nL/side).

For retrograde tracing of the monosynaptic input to PrL CaMKII neurons, a mixture of AAV-CaMKIIα-Cre-WPRE-hGH polyA (AAV2/9, 5.22 × 1012 vector genome/ml), AAV-EF1α-DIO-mCherry-F2A-TVA-WPRE polyA (AAV2/8, ≥2.00 × 1012 vector genome/ml), and AAV- EF1α-DIO-ΔRVG-WPRE-hGH polyA (AAV2/8, ≥2.00 × 1012 vector genome/ml) was injected in the PrL in wildtype rats (1:1:1 ratio, a total of 600 nL). 3 weeks later, RV-EnvA-ΔG- EGFP (300 nL, ≥ 2.00 × 108 IFU/ml) was injected at the same coordinates under the same conditions.

For detecting the dendritic spine in the PrL CaMKII neurons receiving BLA input along with the inhibition of BLA CaMKII neurons, AAV-CaMKIIα-DIO-EGFP-WPREs (AAV2/8, 5.81 × 1012 vector genome/mL) was injected into the PrL (300 nL/side), and a mixture of AAV-hSyn-Cre-WPRE-hGH polyA (AAV2/1, 1.04 × 1013 vector genome/ml) and AAV-CaMKIIα-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry-WPRE polyA (AAV2/9, 5.81 × 1012 vector genome/mL or 1.04 × 1013 vector genome/ml) was injected in the BLA (1:1 ratio, a total of 500 nL/side).

The virus was injected in the PrL (anterior/posterior, +3.2 mm, medial/lateral, ±0.5 mm; dorsal/ventral, -4.0 mm), BLA (anterior/posterior, -2.9 mm; medial/lateral, ±5.2 mm; dorsal/ventral, -8.6 mm), or IL (anterior/posterior, +3.3 mm; medial/lateral, ±0.6 mm; dorsal/ventral, -5.3 mm). All viruses were purchased from BrainVTA (Wuhan, China). We injected the virus (1nL/s) bilaterally using 10 μL microsyringes (Shanghai Gaoge Industrial and Trading Co., Ltd) that were connected to a borosilicate glass capillary. The syringes were connected to a nanoliter microinjection pump (World Precision Instruments) that was controlled by SYS-Micro4 (World Precision Instruments). To prevent virus from diffusing to adjacent brain regions, we maintained the glass capillary for 5 min after insertion and held it for an additional 10 min after the injection.

Fiber photometry

During the memory test, we recorded the fluorescence signal of the CaMKII neurons in the PrL or BLA, using a standard 405/470-nm multi-channel fiber photometry device (Inper Technology, Hangzhou, Zhejiang province, China). A 470 nm laser was used to excite GCaMP fluorescence signals to measure the Ca2+ activity of BLA/PrL neurons, and 405 nm laser served as a control for movement artifacts and fluorescence bleaching. As an appropriate acquisition parameter, the intensity of the laser at the tip of fiber was adjusted to 20-40 μW, and the signal was recorded at 50 Hz. All data were analyzed using custom programs written in Python. To process Ca2+ signals, baseline correction and motion-correction strategies were employed. The 405 nm trace was subtracted from the 470 nm trace using a least-squares regression approach. Then we sorted the recorded data by behavioral trials. For each trial, data were collected between the first and last 10 s of the cue onset. For each trial, Delta F/F ratio = (470 signal – 405 signal) / 405 signal [17]. Graphs and heatmaps were generated in the Jupyter Notebook by plotting data 10 s before and after cue onset. The AUC was calculated in the Jupyter Notebook using the ‘sklearn.metrics auc’ function, analyzing data from 10 s before to 10 s after the cue onset. The trial data were aligned with the cue exposure and presented as an average trace, with shaded areas indicating standard error mean (SEM) fluctuations.

Cocaine self-administration training

Self-administration chambers were equipped with two transparent organic glass walls, two gray aluminum walls, and a steel grid floor. There was one left nosepoke hole and one right nosepoke hole positioned 5 cm above the grid floor. When the rat put its nose in the left nosepoke hole, it would receive a cocaine infusion, simultaneously paired with cues (tone and extinguished light) that remained on for 5 s. We recorded this nosepoke as an “active nosepoke.” A nosepoke in the right hole had no drug or cue consequences and was recorded as an “inactive nosepoke.”

Consistent with our previous research [18], we trained rats for 10 days to self-administer cocaine HCl (Qinghai Pharmaceutical Factory, dissolved in 5% saline; 0.75 mg/kg/infusion) in six 1-h daily sessions (fixed-ratio 1), separated by a 5 min interval. After an active nosepoke, a 40 s timeout period elapsed, during which no drug was available and no cue was presented. The number of cocaine infusions was limited to 20 per session to prevent overdose. Training began at the beginning of the dark cycle with the presentation of a light. During the last three training days, rats with a daily infusion count below 40 were excluded. After cocaine self-administration training, the rats were housed in their homecage for abstinence, handled 1–2 times per week, and allocated into different groups with matched infusion numbers. Food and water were freely available.

Memory test

The memory test consisted of a 1 h session. The test conditions were the same as training, including the tone and light, with the exception that an active nosepoke did not trigger cocaine delivery. The test began at the beginning of the dark cycle with the presentation of a light.

Locomotor test

We equipped locomotor chambers (40 cm × 40 cm × 60 cm, JL Behv-LAG-8) with dark walls and a grid floor. A camera was mounted on top of each chamber. An activity monitoring system recorded the distance traveled, time spent in the center area, and velocity. The test lasted 10 min [19].

Chemogenetic manipulation

Clozapine-N-oxide (CNO; catalog no. HY-17366) was purchased from MedChem Express. Before the intraperitoneal injection, CNO was prepared in 10 mg/mL with dimethylsulfoxide and diluted to 1 mg/mL with saline. For acute chemogenetic intervention, CNO or vehicle (1 mg/kg) was injected intraperitoneally 30 min before the test. For chronic inhibition, we injected CNO or vehicle (1 mg/kg, i.p.) twice daily (8:00-9:00 AM, 8:00-9:00 PM) because the effect duration of CNO is only 10–11 h [20].

Immunofluorescence

We chose 60 min after the test to measure Fos levels in the brain. After the rats were anesthetized with 20% chloral hydrate, they were perfused with 1× PBS and 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA). Brains were sectioned coronally at 20 μm for immunofluorescence.

Brain sections were blocked in 5% bovine serum albumin (BSA) in 1× PBS for 1 h at 37 °C and incubated overnight at 4 °C with rabbit anti-c-fos (1:500, Cell Signaling Technology, catalog no. 2250 s), mouse anti-NeuN (1:500, Millipore, catalog no. MAB377B), or mouse anti-CaMKII (1:500, Abcam, catalog no. ab52476) in 5% BSA in 1× PBS. Sections were rinsed in 1× PBS (4 × 5 min) and incubated for 2 h in the dark at room temperature with donkey anti-rabbit Alexa Fluor 488 (1:500, Invitrogen, catalog no. A-21206) and donkey anti-mouse Alexa Fluor 594 (1:500, Invitrogen, catalog no. A-21203). Sections were rinsed in 1× PBS (4 × 5 min) and mounted on glass slides. We used a fluorescence microscope (Olympus, Tokyo, Japan) with a 20× objective lens.

For detecting the c-Fos positive cells, all cell counting was conducted by a researcher who was blind to experimental groups and analyzed using ImageJ software. At least three sections were selected from each brain region for every rat. The number of Fos-positive cells was automatically counted by applying a uniform threshold for each section from each brain region. The number of dual Fos-positive/CaMKII-positive cells was manually calculated from cells that were selected by ImageJ.

Spine morphometry analysis

The brains were extracted, cut into 50 μm sections, and prepared for confocal microscopy. We used the TCS-SP8 DIVE with a 63 oil-immersion objective (APO CS2 63×, NA = 1.4), a zoom factor of 4, image size (1024 × 1024 pixels, 43.93 × 43.93 μm), with a pinhole set at 1 AU, and a 0.1-µm step size for z-stack image acquisition. Dendritic segments were collected from the secondary or tertiary branches of neurons. We utilized the FilamentTracer module within the Imaris 8 software (Oxford Instruments) to perform three-dimensional reconstructions of dendritic spine morphologies. By utilizing a semi-automated auto-depth function, we analyzed reconstructed dendritic segments with an average length of 20 μm and identified spines. After completing the FilamentTracer creation wizard and generating dendrites and spine objects, all available XTensions are listed under the Tool tab. We used the Imaris Spines Classifier XTension, displaying a list of four default classes: stubby, mushroom, long thin, and filopodial. According to the first rule, all spines with a total length of less than 1 μm are classified as stubby. The second rule states that spines with a mean width of the head greater than the mean width of the neck are classified as mushroom. Long/thin satisfy the third rule, in which the mean width of the neck multiplied by 2 is less than the length of the spine, and the mean width of the neck is less than or equal to the maximum width of the head. All other spines are classified as filopodia [21]. One dendrite was analyzed per neuron, and an average of 25 dendrites were analyzed per animal by a blinded rater. The calculation method for delta density was to first calculate the density of dendritic spines (the number of dendritic spines per unit length of dendrite). This value was then divided by the average spine density. The calculation method for delta nosepoke was the number of nosepokes per rat divided by average nosepoke.

Statistical analysis

We used two-sample unpaired t-tests, Welch’s t-test, one-way repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Tukey’s multiple-comparison post hoc test, Brown-Forsythe ANOVAs followed by Dunnett’s T3 multiple-comparison post hoc test, two-way repeated measures ANOVA followed by the Šídák multiple-comparison post hoc test, and two-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s multiple-comparison post hoc test for the statistical analyses using GraphPad Prism 9.2 software. We used simple linear regression to analyze the correlation between delta spine density and delta nosepokes. The data are expressed as the mean ± SEM. Values of P < 0.05 were considered statistically significant.

Results

Enhancement of remote cocaine memory aligns with enhancement of PrL activity to cues

To investigate the dynamics of PrL activity during cocaine memory expression, we employed an extended-access cocaine self-administration model in rats whereby intravenous cocaine was injected and paired with a tone after rats nosepoked in the active hole (Fig. 1a). Similar to previous research [3], rats acquired cocaine-cue associated memory following 10 days of self-administration training (Fig. 1b). The expression of cocaine memory was manifested by cue-induced active nosepokes, and this memory test was performed 1, 15, or 30 days after training. The results showed that cue-induced nosepoke behavior increased on day 30 compared with day 1, suggesting the enhancement of remote cocaine memory (Fig. 1c). Next, we assessed whether PrL neurons were activated during recent and remote memory expression. We analyzed the c-Fos levels, a marker of neuronal activation, after the behavioral test. We found a parallel change in the density of PrL activated cells with cocaine memory expression. After the memory test on day 1 or 15, the number of c-Fos positive cells per mm [2] did not differ in the test and no test groups. In contrast, the enhancement of remote memory on day 30 markedly increased the density of c-Fos positive cells in the PrL. Additionally, the number of PrL activated cells on day 30 was significantly higher than on days 1 and 15 (Fig. 1d, e). Collectively, these findings indicate that the enhancement of remote cocaine memory can activate the PrL to a greater extent.

Fig. 1: PrL CaMKII neuron activity time-dependently increases along with the enhancement of remote cocaine memory.
figure 1

a Experimental timeline. After 10 days of cocaine self-administration, the rats underwent a 1 h memory test on day 1, 15, or 30. After 1 h following test, rats were perfused for the immunofluorescence of c-Fos. b Number of cocaine infusions during the 10-day training. c Nosepokes on day 30 were higher than on day 1 (n = 11–12 rats). d The density of c-Fos positive cells in the PrL increased on day 30 after the test. The number of PrL c-Fos positive cells after the test on day 30 was higher than on days 1 and 15, respectively (n = 6–8 rats). e Representative images of c-Fos expression in the PrL in the test group. Green: c-Fos. Red: NeuN. Scale bar: 500 μm, 200 μm. f Cells co-labeled with c-Fos and CaMKII in the PrL was higher after the test on day 30 than on day 1 (n = 4 rats). g AAV-CaMKII-GCaMP6s was injected into the PrL, along with the optical fiber in the same location. h Representative photo of GCaMP6s expression in the PrL. Green: GCaMP6s. Blue: DAPI. Scale bar: 200 μm. i Experimental timeline. After 10 days training, fiber photometry was performed during the memory test on day 1, 15, and 30. j–l Area under curve (AUC) of PrL CaMKII neuron Ca2+ signals increased after cue onset (0–10 s) compared with baseline (–10 to 0 s) on day 1(j), day 15 (k), and day 30 (l) (n = 11 rats). Trial-by-trial heatmap representations of Ca2+ signals during the memory test on day 1 (m), day 15 (n), and day 30 (o). p Peri-event plot of average Ca2+ signals aligned to cue exposure from day 1 to day 30. q The AUC change (after-before) was enhanced on day 15 and day 30 compared with day 1. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001, ****P < 0.0001. The data are expressed as the mean ± SEM. Details of the statistical analyses are shown in Supplementary Tables 13.

We next determined the specific subtype of these PrL activated cells, and detected over an 80% proportion of Ca²+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (CaMKII; a marker of glutamatergic neurons [22]) in PrL c-Fos positive cells (Fig. S1a, b). Therefore, we assessed the activity of PrL CaMKII neurons in recent and remote memory, showing that the density of activated CaMKII neurons also showed a time-dependent increase over time (Fig. 1f). Then, to examine the real-time activity of PrL CaMKII neurons during memory expression, we performed in vivo fiber photometry in rats expressing the AAV-CaMKII-GCaMP6s (Fig. 1g, h). After self-administration training, the Ca2+ signal of PrL CaMKII neurons was monitored during the test, in which a nosepoke was paired with a 5-s tone (Fig. 1i). The results showed that cue exposure elicited robust increases in Ca2+ signals in PrL CaMKII neurons during the day 1, 15 and 30 tests (Fig. 1j–p). Additionally, Ca2+ signals on days 15 and 30 were both significantly stronger than day 1, whereas Ca2+ signals did not differ between days 15 and 30 (Fig. 1q). These results suggest that the activity of PrL CaMKII neurons in response to cues time-dependently increases, accompanied by an enhancement of remote cocaine memory.

PrL activity during the early period of consolidation facilitates the enhancement of remote memory

To functionally determine whether PrL CaMKII neurons modulate cocaine memory expression, we employed a chemogenetic method to acutely inhibit their activity 1 day (recent memory) and 30 days (remote memory) post-training. AAV-CaMKII-hM4Di-mCherry was injected in the PrL (Fig. 2a, b). After self-administration training, CNO (1 mg/kg of body weight) or vehicle was administered intraperitoneally 30 min before the test (Fig. 2c–e). We found that acute inhibition of PrL CaMKII neurons did not alter recent memory expression, but it effectively suppressed the expression of remote memory, consistent with previous findings (Fig. 2f) [4]. Notably, the enhancement of remote cocaine memory was markedly attenuated compared with recent one (Fig. 2g). These findings suggest that remote but not recent memory expression relies on PrL CaMKII neurons.

Fig. 2: Inhibition of PrL CaMKII neurons prevents the enhancement and consolidation of remote cocaine memory.
figure 2

a AAV-CaMKII-hM4Di-mCherry was injected in the PrL. b Representative photograph of hM4Di expression in the PrL. Red: hM4Di-mCherry. Blue: DAPI. Scale bar: 1 mm, 20 μm. c Experimental timeline. After 10 days of training, the rats underwent the memory test on day 1 and day 30, along with a CNO or vehicle (1 mg/kg, i.p.) injection 30 min before each test. Number of cocaine infusions (d) and inactive nosepokes (e) during the 10 days of training. f The chemogenetic inhibition of PrL CaMKII neurons only reduced active nosepokes on day 30 (n = 11 rats). g In rats receiving a CNO administration, the number of nosepokes on day 30 was less than on day 1. h Representative photographs of hM4Di-mCherry expression in the PrL. Red: mCherry. Blue: DAPI. Scale bar: 1 mm, 20 μm. Experimental timeline of chronic CNO or vehicle (1 mg/kg, i.p.) administration from day 1 to day 14 (i) or from day 15 to day 28 (j). Number of cocaine infusions (k) and inactive nosepokes (l) during the 10 days of self-administration training. m The chronic inhibition of PrL CaMKII neurons from day 1 to day 14 post training attenuated active nosepokes on day 30 (n = 10–12 rats). Number of cocaine infusions (n) and inactive nosepokes (o) during the 10 days of self-administration training. p The chronic inhibition of PrL CaMKII neurons from day 15 to day 28 post training had no influence on active nosepokes on day 30 (n = 10–17 rats). *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ****P < 0.0001. The data are expressed as the mean ± SEM. Details of the statistical analyses are showed in Supplementary Table 1.

Numerous studies reported that the functional contribution of PrL to remote memory emerged during memory system consolidation [9, 11, 23]. Therefore, we hypothesized that the enhancement of remote cocaine memory stems from memory consolidation through strengthening of PrL CaMKII neurons activity. To test this hypothesis, we utilized chemogenetic inhibition by injecting CNO (1 mg/kg, i.p.) twice daily to repeatedly suppress the activity of PrL CaMKII neurons during system consolidation without affecting the memory expression process (Fig. 2h) [20, 24]. Based on our finding that the pattern of the PrL response to cocaine-related cues was shifted from day 15 (Fig. 1q), we distinguished an early period (day 1–14 post-training) and late period (day 15–28 post-training) to investigate the influence of PrL CaMKII neurons in different consolidation processes on remote memory (Fig. 2i, j). After chronically silencing PrL CaMKII neurons during the early period, we found a pronounced decrease in the number of active nosepokes in the CNO group (Fig. 2k–m). An identical manipulation was performed during the late period, whereas the expression of remote cocaine memory on day 30 was not different between the CNO and vehicle groups (Fig. 2n–p). These results indicate that the activity of PrL CaMKII neurons during the early, but not late, period of consolidation is necessary for remote cocaine memory enhancement.

To exclude the possible confounding effect of the CNO injection itself [25], control rats received injections of AAV-CaMKII-mCherry in the PrL and were trained in the same paradigms, accompanied by administering CNO or vehicle during the early period (Fig. S2a–c). The result showed that injections of CNO alone during the early period did not affect active nosepokes on day 30 (Fig. S2d–f). Thus, these collective findings suggest that memory consolidation contributes to this PrL-involved remote memory enhancement, which is established during the first 2 weeks after training.

The BLA serves as a monosynaptic input to PrL but does not exert a function in memory expression

Strong evidence indicates that during system consolidation, the PrL requires upstream inputs to drive its function in remote memory [9, 11]. Therefore, to identify PrL inputs that may contribute to its progressive activity during consolidation, we applied retrograde trans-synaptic labeling to specifically assess the presynaptic targets of PrL CaMKII neurons. A mixture of AAV-CaMKIIα-Cre, AAV-EF1α-DIO-mCherry-TVA, and AAV- EF1α-DIO-ΔRG was injected into the PrL in wild-type rats (Fig. S3a, b). 21 days later, RV-EnvA-ΔG-EGFP was administered in the same location in all animals (Fig. 3a). We detected the co-expression of RV-EGFP and mCherry neurons in the PrL which indicated starter CaMKII neurons in the PrL (Fig. 3b, c). Robust EGFP labeling was detected in brain regions with known inputs to PrL CaMKII neurons, including the BLA (Fig. 3d), nucleus accumbens core (Fig. S3c) and shell (Fig. S3d), and hippocampus (Fig. S3e), demonstrating direct monosynaptic connections to the PrL CaMKII neurons (Fig. 3d).

Fig. 3: Enhancement of remote cocaine memory does not require BLA CaMKII neurons.
figure 3

a Experimental timeline. To trace the upstream input to PrL CaMKII neurons, an AAV expressing Cre under the CaMKII promotor was co-injected with AAV-EF1α-DIO-mCherry-TVA and AAV-EF1α-DIO-ΔRG in the PrL. 21 days later, RV-EnVA-ΔG-EGFP was injected in the same location. Starter neurons are identified according to the co-expression of mCherry and RV-EGFP. Input neurons only express RV-EGFP. Representative images of starter neurons in the PrL which co-expressed mCherry and EGFP at scales of 500 μm (b) and 20 μm (c). White arrowheads: true starter neurons. Red: mCherry. Green: EGFP. Blue: DAPI. RV-EGFP labeled input neurons in the BLA at scales of 200 μm scale (d) and 20 μm (d, inset). Green: EGFP. Blue: DAPI. Trial-by-trial heatmap representations of the Ca2+ signals in BLA CaMKII neurons during the memory test on day 1(e), day 15 (f), and day 30 (g) (n = 7 rats). The AUC of BLA CaMKII neuron Ca2+ signals increased after cue onset (0–10 s) compared with before cue exposure (–10 to 0 s) on day 1 (h), day 15 (i), and day 30 (g). k Peri-event plot of average Ca2+ signals aligned to cue exposure from day 1 to day 30. l The AUC change (after-before) had no change across test days. m AAV-CaMKII-hM4Di-mCherry was injected in the BLA. n Representative photograph of hM4Di expression in the BLA. Red: hM4Di-mCherry. Blue: DAPI. Scale bar: 200 μm, 20 μm, o Experimental timeline. After 10 days of training, the rats underwent the memory test on day 1 and day 30, along with a CNO or vehicle (1 mg/kg, i.p.) injection 30 min before every test. Number of cocaine infusions (p) and inactive nosepokes (q) during the 10 days of self-administration training. r The chemogenetic inhibition of BLA CaMKII neurons did not influence active nosepokes (n = 6 rats). s In rats receiving a CNO or vehicle injection, both active nosepokes increased on day 30 compared with day 1. **P < 0.01. The data are expressed as the mean ± SEM. Details of the statistical analyses are shown in Supplementary Table 1 and 3.

We selected the BLA for further investigation because it has been shown to be a hub of processing stimulus-cue associations, and its projection is necessary for the PrL to maintain this association information [26, 27]. Hence, we first investigated whether the BLA is activated after cocaine memory expression, finding that the density of BLA c-Fos positive cells was increased across every test (Fig. S4a, b). Unexpectedly, although most activated cells in the BLA were CaMKII type, no difference in the density of BLA activated CaMKII neurons was found over time (Fig. S4c, d). Next, to investigate the real-time activity of BLA CaMKII neurons during cocaine memory expression, we applied fiber photometry. Our results showed that the Ca2+ signals of BLA CaMKII neurons were elevated after cue exposure compared with before cue onset (Fig. 3e–j), suggesting that BLA CaMKII neurons respond to cocaine-related cues during memory expression [28]. However, we did not find time-dependent changes in Ca2+ signals of BLA CaMKII neurons to cue from day 1, 15, to 30 (Fig. 3l). Thus, BLA CaMKII neurons may stably store information about cocaine-cue associations.

To further elucidate the behavioral function of BLA CaMKII neurons in cocaine memory expression, we acutely inhibited BLA CaMKII neurons via chemogenetic method (Fig. 3m–o). After self-administration training, we found that neither recent nor remote memory expression changed after inhibiting BLA CaMKII neurons (Fig. 3p–r). Rats in both CNO and vehicle group presented enhanced nosepokes in the 30-day test compared with day 1 (Fig. 3s). Altogether, our results indicate that despite the stable response of BLA CaMKII neurons to cues is not required for remote cocaine memory enhancement, the BLA may serve as a continual input to the PrL during the consolidation process.

BLA input supports the enhancement of remote memory and PrL spine plasticity during consolidation

Previous evidence suggests that the BLA input during consolidation is critical for supporting PrL function in remote memory [9]. Thus, we hypothesized that the PrL required the BLA projection across system consolidation to facilitate the enhancement of remote cocaine memory. To examine this hypothesis, we injected the retroAAV–hSyn–EGFP–Cre into the PrL and simultaneously injecting the AAV-CaMKII-DIO-hM4Di-mCherry into the BLA, to specifically inhibit BLA CaMKII neurons projecting to the PrL (Fig. 4a–c). The results showed that the chronic silencing of PrL-projecting BLA CaMKII neurons significantly prevented active nosepokes on day 30 (Fig. 4d–g). To identify the behavioral specificity of this effect, we assessed the influence of chronically inhibiting the BLA-PrL circuit on exploratory behavior in an open field (Fig. 4h). We found no change in the total distance traveled (Fig. 4i), average velocity (Fig. 4j), or time spent in the center zone (Fig. 4k) between rats received CNO or vehicle during the early period post-training. These findings demonstrate that nonspecific effects on locomotor activity did not account for the attenuation of remote memory when the BLA-PrL circuit was blocked. When CNO or vehicle (1 mg/kg, i.p.) was administered during the late period (Fig. 4l), we observed no change in cocaine memory expression on day 30 (Fig. 4m–o), and no change in any behaviors in the open field apparatus in these rats (Fig. 4p–s).

Fig. 4: Chronic inhibition of the BLA-PrL circuit during the early period of consolidation prevents the enhancement of remote cocaine memory.
figure 4

a AAV-CaMKII-DIO-hM4Di-mCherry was injected in the BLA, and retroAAV-hSyn-EGFP-cre was injected in the PrL. b Representative images of EGFP-cre expression in the PrL. Green: EGFP. Blue: DAPI. Scale bar: 1 mm (left), 50 μm (right). c Representative images of hM4Di-mCherry expression in the BLA. Red: mCherry. Green: EGFP. Blue: DAPI. Scale bar: 200 μm, 20 μm (c, inset). d Experimental timeline of chronic CNO or vehicle (1 mg/kg, i.p.) administration from day 1 to day 14. Number of cocaine infusions (e) and inactive nosepokes (f) during the 10 days of self-administration training. g Chronic inhibition of BLA-PrL circuit from day 1 to day 14 post training reduced the active nosepokes on day 30 (n = 9–13 rats). h Trajectory path during the locomotor test in the CNO and vehicle groups which received injection during the early period (n = 5–7 rats). Total distance traveled (i), average velocity (j), and time spent in the center zone (k) did not change after chronic inhibition of the BLA-PrL projection during the early period. l Experimental timeline of chronic CNO or vehicle (1 mg/kg, i.p.) administration from day 15 to day 28. Number of cocaine infusions (m) and inactive nosepokes (n) during the 10-day self-administration training. o The number of active nosepokes on day 30 had no difference after chronically blocking the BLA-PrL circuit from day 15 to day 28 post-training (n = 13–14 rats). p Trajectory path during the locomotor test in the CNO and vehicle groups which received injection during the late period (n = 5–6). Total distance traveled (q), average velocity (r), and time spent in the center zone (s) had no change after chronic inhibition of the BLA-PrL projection during late period. *P <0.05. The data are expressed as the mean ± SEM. Details of the statistical analyses are shown in Supplementary Table 1.

To further investigate the specific role of the BLA-PrL circuit, we studied the effect of chronically inhibiting the BLA-infralimbic cortex (IL; a region adjacent to the PrL) circuit. The results showed that chronically silencing the BLA-IL circuit had no significant influence on the enhanced remote cocaine memory on day 30 (Fig. S5). Collectively, these results suggest that the contribution of system consolidation to the enhancement of remote cocaine memory specifically depends on activity of BLA-PrL projection during the early period post-training.

Numerous studies have revealed that PrL function on remote memory results from gradually mature spine plasticity during consolidation, which relies on upstream inputs [29]. Therefore, we hypothesized that the BLA input may drive PrL function on remote cocaine memory through modulating dendritic spines in the PrL. To visualize the structure of dendritic spines in PrL CaMKII neurons receiving BLA projection, we applied a trans-synaptic anterograde method (Fig. 5a). An anterograde trans-synaptic virus AAV1 was infused with Cre-recombinase, and AAV1-hSyn-Cre was injected in the BLA. Simultaneously, AAV-CaMKII-DIO-EGFP was administered in the PrL, leading to the expression of green fluorescence in PrL CaMKII neurons which received BLA input. Furthermore, to simultaneously investigate the effect of the BLA projection on PrL spine plasticity during memory consolidation, AAV-CaMKII-hM4Di-mCherry was injected into the BLA in these rats. After chronically silencing the BLA CaMKII neurons through administering CNO or vehicle (1 mg/kg, i.p.) during the early period post-training, rats were sacrificed to detect spines of PrL neurons receiving BLA projection (Fig. 5b-d). We performed high-fidelity three-dimensional reconstruction of dendritic spine morphologies and differentiated various types of spines, including nonmature spines (e.g., filopodia and long thin type) and mature spines (e.g., mushroom type and stubby type; Fig. 5e–g) [30]. We found that the spine density of PrL neurons receiving BLA input significantly decreased after chronically silencing BLA glutamatergic neurons (Fig. 5h). The stratification of spines by morphological subtype showed that this effect was driven by a selective decrease in filopodia-shaped spines and mushroom-shaped spines (Fig. 5i-l). Next, we examined whether there was a relationship between mushroom-type spines and cocaine memory expression. Through analyzing the correlation between delta spine density and delta nosepokes, we found a significant positive correlation between mushroom-shaped spines and cocaine memory (Fig. 5m). Altogether, these findings suggest that the maturation of PrL dendritic spines during the early period of consolidation requires the BLA input, thereby facilitating the enhancement of remote cocaine memory.

Fig. 5: Chronic inhibition of the BLA input during the early period of consolidation impairs the dendritic spine density and maturation of PrL CaMKII neurons.
figure 5

a A mixture of AAV1-hSyn-Cre and AAV-CaMKII-hM4Di-mCherry was injected in the BLA, along with an injection of AAV-CaMKII-DIO-EGFP in the PrL. b Experimental timeline of chronic CNO or vehicle (1 mg/kg, i.p.) administration from day 1 to day 14. Number of cocaine infusions (c) and inactive nosepokes (d) during the 10 days of self-administration training. e Representative image showing the EGFP expression in the PrL neurons receiving BLA input. Green: EGFP. Scale bar: 10 μm. Representative three-dimensional dendritic spine reconstruction and classification in the vehicle (f) and CNO (g) groups. Scale bar: 2 μm. Red: stubby type. Green: mushroom type. Blue: long thin type. Purple: filopodia type. Dendritic spine density for all spines (h) and stratified by filopodia (i), long thin type (j), mushroom type (k), and stubby type (l). Of these, a reduction of spine density was observed in all spines, filopodia-shaped spines, and mushroom-shaped spines (n = 5 rats). m The delta density of mushroom-shaped spines showed a significant positive correlation with the delta nosepoke behaviors. n Cocaine memory expression after cue exposure showed time-dependent enhancement, relying on the progressively increased and mature dendrite spine in PrL CaMKII neuron. And these neurons required the sustained BLA input during memory consolidation, especially during the early period of system consolidation process. *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001, ****P < 0.0001. The data are expressed as the mean ± SEM. Details of the statistical analyses are shown in Supplementary Table 4.

Discussion

The present study provides compelling evidence that the enhancement of remote cocaine memory is driven by memory system consolidation. Using an extended cocaine self-administration model in rats, we found that remote cocaine-cue associated memory expression exhibited an enhancement. During this process, we observed a time-dependent strengthening of the response of PrL CaMKII neurons to cocaine-related cues. Chemogenetic suppression also revealed that memory consolidation drove the enhancement of remote cocaine memory through PrL CaMKII neurons, and the early period after training was the major stage for effective intervention. Furthermore, the function of PrL CaMKII neurons in remote memory required the BLA input during the early period of consolidation, which can promote the maturation of PrL dendritic spines, mainly mushroom-shaped spines. These data extend previous findings and showed the contribution of system consolidation to the enhancement of remote cocaine memory through PrL CaMKII neurons receiving the BLA input (Fig. 5n).

Our results indicate that the association information attached to the cocaine-related cues was time-dependently enhanced, but not attenuated, over time. Notably, a similar phenomenon has been reported that cue-induced drug craving (i.e., an intense desire for the effects of a drug) progressively increased during long-term abstinence, which was termed as the incubation of cue-induced drug craving [31, 32]. The incubated drug craving is a critical contributor to drug relapse and generalizes across various drugs [33, 34]. Many human studies have identified this phenomenon in individuals with a history of taking nicotine [35], methamphetamine [36], alcohol [37], or cocaine [6]. Emerging evidence support that drug craving stemmed from past experiences about drug use which can be encoded and stored in memory, suggesting that memory serves as the foundation of drug craving [2, 13, 38, 39]. Here, our results further demonstrate homogeneity between the enhancement of remote drug-cue associated memory and the incubation of drug craving. Thus, we speculate that the stable memory is a prerequisite for persistent drug craving during abstinence, and the enhancement of remote drug-cue memory driven by memory consolidation leads to the strengthened cocaine-cue association information, finally eliciting increased cue-induced drug craving upon cue re-exposure [40]. Although direct evidence is still scarce, many indirect studies have shown that disrupting the memory reconsolidation process can attenuate cue-induced drug craving [15, 39]. Therefore, it is worthy to explore the ways in which the enhancement of drug memories causes an abnormal intensification of drug craving.

Memory system consolidation typically facilitates recent memory to maintain and to turn into stable over time, whereas remote memory tends to become weaker through a process of forgetting [1]. In contrast, our data reveal that memory consolidation can drive remote cocaine memory to an abnormal state, that is expression enhancement, similar to previous finding [8]. This kind of remote memory enhancement generalizes to other types of memory, such as the natural reward-cue associated memory [41] and fear memory [42]. Therefore, it is reasonable to suspect that the role of system consolidation in remote memory enhancement is neither attributable to the effect of the addictive drug per se, nor correlated with stimulus valence (i.e., appetitive or aversive). One possible factor affecting the function of consolidation is memory strength, in which high-intensity learning would induce stronger expression of remote memory than low-intensity learning [43]. Here, we applied an extended self-administration training paradigm whereby rats underwent 6-h sessions per day, and every nosepoke could trigger a simultaneous cocaine injection until the maximum number of infusions was reached. Through this extended training, the cue can get associated with the cocaine reward effect and then acquire the ability to elicit seeking behaviors like the cocaine, which can be named as the “association” [44]. The strength of association between cocaine and the cue may be a contributor to the increased remote memory expression, because the rats which were trained with a rapid infusion rate (5 s), but not a slow (90 s), showed a more pronounced enhancement of remote cocaine memory [45]. Additional findings showed that in rats cue extinction session (repetitive exposure to drug-related cues without injecting the drug) during abstinence reduced the remote drug memory enhancement [46], possibly due to that extinction session weakened the association between the drug and related cues and blocked the consolidation of drug-cue associated memory. Hence, we hypothesize that the over-strengthened cocaine-cue association during learning would be abnormally consolidated over long periods of time, making behavioral responses to related cues gradually increase. This may explain why individuals with substance use disorders or those who have experienced traumatic events have a higher risk of relapse or suffer from post-traumatic stress disorder [47, 48]. In turn, the early period of consolidation represents a potential time window for blunting remote memory enhancement. Our results showed that chronic inhibition during the early period reduced the cue-induced behavioral response on day 30 post-training. This finding aligns with previous research that the initial 2 weeks post-learning is critical for remote memory [49], suggesting the therapeutic value of preventing system consolidation to reduce the drug relapse.

The role of neocortex in remote memory is not fixed but rather undergoes an evolving process during memory system consolidation [50]. Recent investigations have further revealed a time-dependent maturation process of PrL function in memory which required inputs from various brain regions including medial entorhinal cortex, hippocampus, as well as BLA [9], showing a greater influence on remote memory [23]. In the present study, we found that the function of PrL glutamatergic neurons on remote cocaine memory enhancement was established during the consolidation process. This was supported by two key findings. First, we observed an increase in Ca2+ signals after cue onset from day 1 to day 30 post-training, indicating a gradually greater ability of the PrL to process the information about cocaine related cues. Consistently, previous findings also showed a pronounced increase in the encoding of related cues information after prolonged abstinence from cocaine [51]. Second, our results revealed that chronic chemogenetic inhibition of PrL CaMKII neurons during the early period significantly reduced the enhancement of remote cocaine memory, suggesting the behavioral function of PrL necessitates a temporal period for maturation. However, the experimental design of our study exclusively employed male rats, which should be noted. Given that female rats exhibit stronger cocaine seeking after abstinence compared to male, whether the PrL would play a different role in the enhancement of remote cocaine memory in female rats merits more explorations [52, 53].

The BLA serves as a focal point for drug memory [54], where sensory and emotional signals are processed and integrated [55]. Our study found an increase in Ca2+ signals in BLA CaMKII neurons following exposure to cocaine-related cues. However, unlike the PrL, BLA activity did not show temporal changes during system consolidation, and the acute inhibition of BLA CaMKII neurons did not affect the expression of cocaine memory. These results imply that the BLA may primarily integrate and store the information about cocaine and related cues, while this stored association information in the BLA did not undergo enhancement during prolonged abstinence and thus it is not necessary for the increased behavioral response to cocaine-related cues. Consistent results were also reported, in which pharmacological inhibition of the BLA had no influence on cue-induced cocaine seeking behaviors at remote time [56].

Our findings highlight that the sustained input from the BLA to PrL during the early period of consolidation contributes to enhancement of remote cocaine memory. These processes are accompanied by dynamic dendritic spine plasticity on PrL neurons defined by afferent connections [29, 57]. Here, we found that blocking the BLA glutamatergic projection during the early period of consolidation decreased dendrite spine density in PrL CaMKII neurons receiving BLA input. Among these, the mushroom-shaped spines possibly served as the primary substrate for the elevation of activity of PrL during system consolidation, and appeared to be necessary for the enhancement of remote cocaine memory. This is similar to previous research that PrL neurons displayed more mature and stable synaptic connections during abstinence [58]. Furthermore, the BLA input to PrL also resulted in the transmission of cocaine-cue association information [59]. This transmission possibly has its behavioral significance that the learned object values encoded by the prefrontal cortex can be retained for at least several months and are resistant to interference [60]. Accordingly, we propose that the BLA may function as a cache that integrates the strong association between cocaine and related cues, and during system consolidation BLA can convey this information to PrL for long-term storage. Remarkably, during the early period post-training, more mature and stable dendritic spine plasticity which occurred in PrL neurons receiving BLA input possibly makes the association information abnormally consolidated, inducing stronger response when cue exposure at remote time and leading to enhancement of remote cocaine memory. Following that, the NAcc may be a promising output for investigating the ways in which consolidated cocaine-cue association information flows from the BLA to PrL for persistent storage, and then to downstream regions for directing higher behavioral responses to cues. This is supported by extensive findings that the optogenetic inhibition of NAcc neurons receiving PrL input can attenuate cue-induced drug seeking at abstinence day 45 [5].

In summary, we delineated that memory system consolidation drives the enhancement of remote cocaine memory through stepwise mature dendritic spines plasticity and elevated response to cue in PrL neurons receiving BLA input, and identified the early period of consolidation as an essential time window for preventing the enhanced expression of remote cocaine memory.