Introduction

Anxiety [1] disorders are a major public health problem. They are the most frequent classes of mental disorders in Western societies, for which they are among the leading cause of disability [2] and come at huge individual and societal cost [3]. Anxiety is also comorbid with a variety of medical conditions, exacerbating symptoms, hampering recovery, and increasing the risk for other mental disorders, such as alcoholism and depression. Even when anxiety symptoms do not reach the criteria for a disorder, they can cause misery and poor health [3].

Unfortunately, our understanding of anxiety symptoms remains limited, and the treatment of anxiety disorders represents a significant challenge to mental health [4, 5]. Current drug treatments often have suboptimal efficacy as well as unwanted side effects [5]. This situation reflects the difficulty of translational research, which has struggled to capitalize on the array of new technologies to develop efficient treatments. It also points, to some extent, to the practical challenge of developing animal models of mental disorders [6]. While animal models of anxiety provide critical insights into basic defense-survival mechanisms [4], promising novel treatments derived from animal models too often end up being ineffective in humans, and none of them have led to significant improvements to the current armamentarium of anxiolytic drugs, which has been stagnant for several decades [7]. This lack of effectiveness has prompted the pharmaco-industry to close or downsize research on mental illnesses [8]. Ultimately, this grim picture signals the need for a new approach to translational research to improve the synergy between basic science and clinical science.

In this paper, we argue that experimental models of anxiety in nonclinical, unmedicated humans can revitalize treatment discovery. The “experimental psychopathology” approach can bridge basic research in animals and clinical studies, as well as guide and constrain hypotheses about the nature of psychopathology, treatment mechanisms, and treatment targets [9, 10].

This paper focuses on experimental models of anxiety, as opposed to fear or stress. Of prime consideration when developing experimental models of psychopathology is the nature of symptoms. Fear and anxiety are different features of anxiety disorders [11], with a distinct underlying neurobiology [12]. Fear is a phasic response to a well-defined and identifiable proximal threat, whereas anxiety is a more sustained aversive state generated by uncertain future threat [11, 13]. Fear is above all a behavioral response that mobilizes the organism to act (fight or flight) in the face of a life-threatening situation. By contrast, anxiety is a feeling of apprehension, a perceived sense of unpredictability, and aversive anticipation [13, 14]. While great progress has been made to elucidate the neurobiology of fear, thanks to robust translational experimental models, such as Pavlovian fear conditioning, comparatively much less is known about anxiety, partly because of the difficulty in modeling its symptoms in animals. More than fear, anxiety involves complex cognitive and executive processes mediated by high-order brain neurocircuits that are strikingly more developed in humans than animals, limiting the usefulness of animal models. Psychosocial stress also implicates complex cognitive processes. However, although the distinction between anxiety and psychosocial stress is not straightforward, there are important differences between these two emotional states. Experimentally, anxiety is usually induced by anticipation of an aversive event (e.g., shock [15]), whereas psychosocial stress is evoked by social-evaluative situations (e.g., Trier Social Stress Test [16], Montreal Imaging Stress Task [17]). Research on anxiety and stress focuses on different neurobiological mechanisms, limbic structures [4, 12], and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal gland axis [18], respectively. Both anxiety paradigms and stress induce negative affective states, but anxiety differs from stress in terms of the specific physiological responses and psychological experience (e.g., hypervigilance and attention bias are specific to anxiety) [19], as well as hormonal changes (social stressors increase cortisol more robustly and reliably than anxiety-induction procedures). In addition, during psychosocial stress, the cognitive tasks themselves are used as stressors (the independent variable) and the dependent variables are the autonomic, hormonal, and neural responses. The underlying cognitive processes are not the focus of interest per se. This contrasts with studies that are designed to study the neurocognitive mechanisms that drive anxiety [20,21,22,23,24,25].

In this paper, we (1) briefly discuss the advantages and limitations of animal models and argue for the development of human models of anxiety, (2) present a rationale for the experimental psychopathology approach, (3) describe a neuroscience systems approach to experimental psychopathology in humans to investigate pathological anxiety and its treatments, (4) provide empirical illustrations of this approach, and finally (5) review issues that need to be addressed in future studies.

Animal and human models of anxiety

Experimental models and translational research are necessary to improve our understanding of anxiety, anxiety disorders, and their treatments. Of all the mental disorders, anxiety disorders are arguably most amenable to translation because, unlike many psychiatric symptoms, fear and anxiety are highly tractable in the laboratory. These defense-survival responses to threat can be readily induced and measured in animals and in humans, and their underlying neurocircuits are well-conserved across species [4, 12].

Why do we need to model anxiety in humans, given that (1) basic research in animal models on neurobiological mechanisms can go far beyond research in humans, and (2) clinical research comparing healthy controls with anxiety patients can identify variations from the norms? We argue that human models of anxiety are necessary for the same reasons we rely on animal models: to enhance basic knowledge and theories, define functional norms to help identify pathological mechanisms and treatment targets, and to screen new treatments. Animal and human models, however, have their own specific advantages, limitations, and can address distinct questions. They are complementary, but for progress to be made, their differing scopes, strengths, and weaknesses must be acknowledged.

Animal vs. human experimental models

The advantages of animal models over human models include the availability of more invasive experiments (but see ref. [26]) and of more advanced technical tools (e.g., optogenetic techniques). Research in animal models has advanced the characterization of functional neurocircuits and the underlying neurobiology, and the gene-to-phenotype relationship [27, 28]. Animal models are also essential at the early stages of drug development to establish safety, pharmacokinetics, and early evidence of target effectiveness [29].

However, animal models have also important limitations. They have failed to deliver clinically effective psychopharmacological treatments for anxiety disorders [30, 31] and have limited application to test novel treatments (e.g., mindfulness, neurostimulation). This poor predictive validity of clinical efficacy has multiple causes, including poor construct validity (e.g., in modeling subjective experiences) and the difficulty of modeling the cognitive and behavioral symptoms listed under the major classification systems (e.g., DSM-5) [26, 32]. As a result, the features of human anxiety modeled in animals can be fuzzy. In the defense of animal models, a key hurdle to their development is the current inadequate nosology of mental disorders based on phenomenology (i.e., patients’ reports, signs) rather than the underlying biology, unknown pathogenic mechanisms, absence of biomarkers and recognized highly penetrant genetic variants, clinical heterogeneity, and sex differences in brain structures and functions [6, 22]. In fact, animal models are expected to achieve greater validity and stronger clinical relevance, when the critical features of anxiety and anxiety disorders (biological markers, genetic makeup, and pathophysiology) will become better defined, and the exploration of the mechanisms of behavior in animals more sophisticated, owing to the remarkable advances in basic neuroscience tools and methods.

However, the one irreducible issue concerns the cognitive, emotional, and behavioral traits that are unique to humans [4]. While animal models have been instrumental in elucidating defense-survival mechanisms, these defense-survival mechanisms are only one component of a complex system that contributes to human anxiety. Basic defense mechanisms interact with higher-order cognitive and executive functions mediated by neocortical circuits that are uniquely developed in humans [33]. Above all, psychiatry is the field of medicine devoted to pathologies of the mind, which is the most complex and elusive faculty of the human being. The human mind harbors conscious and unconscious experiences and is the origin of conceptual abstraction and symbolic communication that cannot be assessed in animals [4, 26]. In addition, the neural architecture of the brain is far more complex in humans than in animals, and anxiety disorders often reflect disturbance of evolutionary recent circuits. The neurocircuitry of anxious feelings, mental representations, and emotion regulation in humans implicates prefrontal areas, which are uniquely developed in humans. Many psychiatric symptoms (e.g., worries) cannot be convincingly evaluated in animals, and while some domains of functioning (abnormal social behavior, working memory, and attention control) can be modeled in animals, the analogy is only an approximation [4, 6].

Human models can address these limitations. Experimental models in humans provide insights into the core subjective, cognitive, and self-regulatory aspects of anxiety, which are responsible for functional impairment and bring patients to the clinic. Experimental psychopathology can characterize what is behaviorally and cognitively deviant from the norms in anxiety disorders. Human models are valuable tools to identify potential pathophysiological mechanisms and treatment targets [34,35,36]. Clearly, experimental psychopathology cannot achieve the detailed analysis of brain function that is possible in animal research. However, in coming years, improvement in neuroimaging techniques and computational psychiatry will enhance the yield of human research [37].

Human experimental models vs. clinical research

Human models of anxiety in healthy individuals can also inform and complement clinical research with patients. This is particularly important since research in patients is complex, costly, and challenging with regard to recruitment and confounding factors, such as comorbidity and drug treatment [38]. Fundamentally, experimental models in humans can test reciprocal causal and correlational effects of anxiety on cognition and executive functions [20]. They can also help disentangle the role of anxiety in other conditions that are often comorbid with anxiety (see ref. [22]).

Experimental models in healthy subjects also present an advantage of scale in that they can leverage the benefit of relatively easy recruitment of participants (relative to psychiatric patients) to speed up research and widen its scope for an in-depth investigation of the multiple faceted aspects of anxiety.

In addition, a most promising application of human models relates to treatment, providing some understanding on how treatments work [34, 39]. In addition, human models could be instrumental for screening novel anxiolytics [35, 36], adding specificity to the high sensitivity of animal models (see Step 5: treatment implications). Experimental psychopathology may also be an instructive first step to test non-pharmacological treatments that cannot be assessed in animals, such as cognitive and behavioral treatments (e.g., mindfulness) or neuromodulation [40].

Finally, because anxiety disorders are developmental brain disorders and because the manifestation of anxiety is different in children and in adults [41], experimental psychopathology affords the opportunity to implement a developmental perspective for anxiety research.

Experimental psychopathology: definition and validity

Experimental psychopathology is conducted in healthy individuals, and employs experimental models of psychopathology (e.g., anxiety) to gain insights into normal and abnormal behaviors and their underlying neurobiological mechanisms [42, 43]. The validity of experimental models of anxiety in healthy humans is consistent with the dimensional conceptualization of psychopathology [44, 45], which assumes a continuum from normal to pathological anxiety, and therefore a similar continuity for the underlying mechanisms. While the conceptualization of anxiety disorders in terms of dimensions or categories has long been debated [46, 47], the dimensional view is espoused by preclinical [48] and clinical research [45]. That anxiety disorders result from inappropriate activation or exaggeration of otherwise adaptive responses to threat provides the rationale behind the experimental psychopathology approach. The same anxiety system that is perturbed in anxiety disorders is also activated by anxiety challenges in normal individuals, albeit in less extreme, persistent, or incapacitating forms [24, 25, 49, 50] (see also section ‘Step 3: Studies of the interplay of anxiety and cognition in healthy subjects'). Fear-conditioning studies are a validation of this view. Indeed, many of the neural structures underlying normative fear mechanisms in healthy subjects have been implicated in pathological anxiety, especially symptoms that are fear-specific, such as response to phobic stimuli [51]. It follows that normative neuromechanisms of anxiety should provide a blueprint for the search of pathological mechanisms and drive hypotheses about neurocircuit dysfunction in pathological anxiety. However, while the experimental psychopathology approach is consistent with the dimensional view of anxiety, it remains a hypothesis, and as such, it is falsifiable. If it turns out that normative and pathological mechanisms differ, the dimensional view of psychopathology will have to be revisited.

A recipe for experimental psychopathology research on anxiety

Anxiety is multifaceted and cannot be expected to be recapitulated in a single model. Like animal models, the intent of human models is not to mimic the whole disease [6], but to develop specific tests of behavioral and psychological operations relevant to the symptoms and treatment of the target disorder. Findings informing how experimentally induced anxiety affects these operations help generate hypotheses about biomarkers and the underlying neurobiological dysfunction and narrow the search for treatment targets for the specific modeled symptoms.

While studies of fear-related defense mechanisms examine short-duration responses to threat cues (e.g., Pavlovian fear conditioning), studies focusing on anxiety examine sustained aversive states. Anxiety-induction procedures include situations such as anxiogenic pharmacological challenges (e.g., 7.5% CO2, CCK4) [52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59], darkness [60,61,62], or threat of unpredictable aversive stimuli (e.g., shock) [15, 63, 64]. So far, most studies have investigated defensive mechanisms in idle subjects (i.e., subjects not involved in a complex task) and have enhanced our understanding of the clinical relevance of these anxiety states [65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72] and their underlying neurobiology [53, 73,74,75,76,77,78,79,80,81,82,83].

However, this type of investigation of sustained anxiety, which focuses uniquely on emotional expression, is limited in scope and does not fully capitalize on the advantages of experimental models in humans. Indeed, the cognitive aspects of anxiety state are for the most part ignored. This lapse is an issue since emotional and cognitive processes are not organized separately, but contribute jointly to ongoing behavior [84], and subjective experience relies on cognitive processes such as attention and working memory [85]. In addition, cognitive formulations of anxiety provide elaborated theoretical models of psychopathology [86,87,88,89] and define the processes targeted by successful cognitive therapies. These theories emphasize the role of information processing (i.e., cognition) in the etiology and maintenance of anxiety disorders. In overanxious individuals, faulty perception, encoding, storage, retrieval, interpretation, control, and action interfere with ongoing goals, while deficits in executive function impair self-regulation (emotion regulation) [87, 90,91,92,93]. However, we currently have little mechanistic understanding of the impact of anxiety on these processes and their dysregulation in anxiety disorders. It is to study these cognitive processes that experimental psychopathology can be most beneficial. A better characterization of the dynamics between anxiety and cognition with the objective of identifying core cognitive processes that are vulnerable to anxiety could significantly improve our understanding of the pathological mechanisms.

Investigators have long used systems neuroscience to elucidate neurocircuits mediating behavioral plasticity and pharmacological effects [94]. This approach begins with the characterization of the functional neural architecture that mediates the behavior under scrutiny to subsequently identify where and how plasticity occurs in response to experimental manipulations. The systems neuroscience approach has been used with great success to uncover the underlying mechanisms of simple defense responses to threat in animals [94, 95] and in humans [96]. We argue that this approach can be applied to investigate how anxiety affects complex perceptual, cognitive, and behavioral processes in experimental psychopathology studies in humans.

The overall strategy is to combine a primary cognitive or behavioral task with experimentally induced anxiety. Its implementation follows five steps:

  1. (1)

    Select anxiety-relevant cognitive or behavioral operations (e.g., attention bias for threat) and tasks (e.g., dot probe) to probe these processes,

  2. (2)

    Identify the underlying neurocircuits via connectivity/activation nodes in healthy volunteers,

  3. (3)

    Examine the impact of experimental anxiety on the targeted operations and the underlying neurocircuits in healthy volunteers,

  4. (4)

    Utilize findings from step 3 to generate and constrain hypotheses about the mechanisms of pathophysiology and neurocircuit dysfunction in anxious patients, and

  5. (5)

    Finally, evaluate the mechanisms of treatment efficacy and screen putative anxiolytic treatments in healthy individuals exposed to experimental anxiety.

Step 1: functional behavioral and cognitive domains and markers of anxiety

The first step is to select anxiety-relevant psychological and behavioral processes or constructs. One productive approach is to base this selection on findings from clinical research, which compares patients with anxiety disorders with individuals without psychiatric disorders. These studies have identified cognitive, executive, behavioral, neurobiological, and electrophysiological abnormalities in anxiety patients. The results of these studies have generated important theoretical models and psychological constructs that underscore key cognitive, executive, and behavioral variables presumed to play a role in the origins and maintenance of anxiety disorders. These include negative bias in information processing [92, 97, 98], worry [99, 100], emotional dysregulation [101], behavioral inhibition [102], and avoidance [103]. There are several established tasks and measures to investigate these constructs. For example, information- processing biases can be evaluated using event-related potentials (ERPs) in response to sensory stimuli [104,105,106] or behavioral responses on cognitive tasks, such as the dot probe, visual search paradigms, and the Stroop test [92]. These tasks have enhanced our understanding of how anxiety captures attention, but they have also generated conflicting theories regarding the nature and the stage of information-processing deficits in anxiety [92]. Resolution of these conflicts can be facilitated with studies of experimental anxiety in healthy subjects.

Another promising and more systematic approach relies on the Research Domain Criteria (RdoC) project, which seeks to relate dysfunction of specific neurobiological systems to symptoms [45]. The RdoC is organized around fundamental functional constructs that are studied along the full range of variation, from normal to abnormal, across multiple units of analysis. These constructs are grouped into higher-level functional domains and are investigated using well-validated paradigms that have been selected by experts in the field. Using a RdoC approach, experimental psychopathology seeks to better understand how anxiety changes behavior–brain relationships, and then relates these changes to pathological anxiety. One could argue that anxiety spans several RDoC constructs, the “potential threat” construct of the negative valence systems, and various constructs of the cognitive systems, including attention, perception, cognitive control, and working memory. For example, attentional bias for threat falls into the cognitive system and the negative valence system. From a RDoC perspective, experimental psychopathology explores the interaction between the domains of potential threat and various cognitive system constructs. The RDoC represents a valuable framework for organizing experimental psychopathology research, as it provides the scientific rationale to select constructs and validated tasks to probe these constructs. However, the list of constructs is not exhaustive, and it could be worthwhile to identify additional anxiety-relevant cognitive constructs (e.g., affective decision-making) [107]. This could be accomplished by following the example of research on the negative symptoms of schizophrenia. The MATRICS (Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia) initiative was designed to develop a consensus cognitive battery to stimulate the development of new drugs that target cognitive deficits in schizophrenia [108]. A panel of experts was put together to identify the relevant cognitive domains and constructs and select a battery of cognitive tests relevant to schizophrenia. A similar approach would greatly benefit the study of the core cognitive symptoms of anxiety.

Step 2: normative mechanisms in healthy subjects

This step belongs to the domain of cognitive neuroscience, which seeks to understand the neurocircuits of cognitive processes. Step 2 of the experimental psychopathology approach consists of identifying, in healthy individuals, the neural circuits underlying the behavioral/cognitive constructs selected in step 1. Neuroimaging techniques are the basic tools to examine these questions. They include most commonly magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), but also magnetoencephalography (MEG), and less frequently positron emission tomography (PET). Whereas information can be gleaned from structural brain data, functional data provide more direct information about the cognitive processes, since they record changes in neural activity that co-occur with changes in behavior. Two basic measures are supplied by fMRI: regional activation and functional connectivity. Most of the analyses, up till now, are correlational in nature. For example, working memory is a cognitive construct which has been “associated” with activations within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and parietal cortex. These regional activations are interpreted as forming the neural circuit that supports working memory. Naturally, the same is true for the functional connectivity measure.

Extended efforts have been made to formulate mathematical algorithms that could generate causal models. Two complementary methods, Granger causality (G-causality [109]) and dynamic causal modeling [110] have been developed, but have not been extensively applied. The reasons for this include the many assumptions and requirements needed for their successful application, and their complexity. A more in-depth discussion of these techniques is outside the scope of this review but see ref. [111]. Computational models of behavioral and neural processes are also powerful approaches to understand the underlying mechanisms that drive anxiety pathology. The idea is to build mathematical models of normal and abnormal behaviors to identify the components that drive pathology [112]. More recently, the introduction of machine-learning tools, while large data sets become publicly available, may change the landscape of brain–behavior neuroscience research. For the perspective of this review, machine-learning tools offer the possibility of exploiting a large array of behavioral and neuroimaging variables to not only refine the understanding of neural circuits, but also discover novel mechanisms underlying specific behavioral processes.

Step 3: studies of the interplay of anxiety and cognition in healthy subjects

Once the behavioral/cognitive process (Step 1) and its underlying neurocircuits (Step 2) are delineated, Step 3 assesses the effect of experimentally induced anxiety on these factors. Step 3 is the critical step that determines the relevance of the cognitive construct and the associated neural circuit to anxiety. Promising results from this step are then followed up in steps 4 and 5. However, negative results signal the end of this specific investigation. Step 3 is the stage of experimental psychopathology that departs from but is the link to clinical research. In Step 3, cognitive tasks and their behavioral and neural signatures are explored in healthy individuals in a control condition and under a condition of experimentally induced sustained anxiety. Anxiety-related changes in cognitive performance and the underlying neural substrates are then hypothesized to be markers of anxiety in general, and are expected to be relevant to clinical anxiety, which is what steps 4 and 5 will test. Figure 1 illustrates a hypothetical scenario where a neurocircuit underlying response to a cognitive task is affected by induced anxiety in healthy individuals. A network of three structures (ROI1, ROI2, and ROI3) is implicated functionally in the task. During an anxiety-induction procedure, another structure, ROIx is activated and alters connectivity in the network, between ROI1 and ROI2. These changes in neural activity and connectivity would be expected to be implicated in pathological anxiety.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Diagram illustrating the five-step experimental approach. Step 1: selection of a clinically relevant construct (e.g., attention bias for threat) and tasks (e.g., dot probe) that probe the construct; Step 2: characterization of the response (output) and the underlying neurobiology associated with the task (input) probing the construct in healthy subjects. The response can be a behavioral, cognitive, a pharmacological, or electrophysiological measure. The regions of interest ROI1, ROI2, and ROI3 represent three hypothetical structures that mediate the behavior tested by the task. The arrows represent hypothetical directional connectivity among these structures; Step 3: the task is combined with an anxiety-induction procedure to determine how anxiety affects the response and the underlying neurocircuits. In this example, a structure ROIx affects the connectivity between ROI1 and ROI2. In Step 4, the task is tested in patients to determine whether the same processes and neurocircuits affected by experimental anxiety in healthy subjects are also implicated in the patients. Patients can be tested with or without an anxiety-induction procedure. Step 5: healthy subjects are tested as in Step 3 (task+induced anxiety) but are also given a treatment to examine the mechanisms of treatment responses or to screen putative anxiolytic treatments (do they affect the response and do they affect the same anxiety–neurocircuit identified in Step 2?)

Ultimately, the aim of experimentally induced anxiety is to uncover markers of anxiety that can lead to treatments. There has been a steady increase in studying the interplay of anxiety and cognition in recent years. The great majority of studies use the threat of unpredictable shocks [20, 21, 113,114,115,116,117,118,119] (also reviewed in ref. [120]) or 7.5% CO2 inhalation [22,23,24,25]. The trend that emerges is that anxiety induced by threat of shock can not only have detrimental but also beneficial effects, depending on the nature of the task (reviewed in ref. [120]. Generally, threat of shock facilitates early sensory processing, the detection of threat information, and interference from distractors. It impairs short-term memory but facilitates long-term memory. It does not have a uniform effect on inhibitory control, impairing cognitive inhibition in emotional Stroop tests, while facilitating response inhibition in go–nogo tasks. Breathing 7.5% CO2 increases the measures of hypervigilance, such as orienting and alerting [24, 25], and negative interpretation bias [121]. Overall, many of these effects mirror the effect of clinical anxiety on cognitive processes.

Therefore, the next step (Step 4) is to evaluate the use of these findings in healthy subjects as markers of clinical anxiety.

Step 4: mechanisms of pathophysiology

One objective of experimental psychopathology is to guide hypotheses about dysregulated cognitive processes and the underlying neurocircuits in pathological anxiety. The dimensional perspective of psychopathology predicts that pathological anxiety is associated with perturbations in some of the processes and neural mechanisms engaged by experimental anxiety. In the example we provide above (Fig. 1), one would hypothesize that the structure ROIx and the connectivity between ROI1 and ROI2 are potentially implicated in pathological anxiety.

What is the nature of the dysfunction that should be expected? From a dimensional perspective, the mechanisms that exacerbate responses to anxiety in healthy individuals should be amplified in anxiety patients (i.e., amplified excitatory mechanisms, and weakened inhibitory mechanisms). However, neuroadaptation following chronic anxiety may change the nature of these responses and the underlying neurocircuits over time. Consistent with this view, while studies generally show increased defensive reactivity and increased amygdala activity in pathological anxiety [72, 122], instances of hyporeactivity in patients are not infrequent [123, 124]. The current interpretation of these opposing effects is that they reflect distinct symptoms. For example, while pathological anxiety tends to be associated with exaggerated defensive responses as measured with startle [66, 67], increased illness chronicity and comorbidity are associated with blunted reactivity [124]. A similar pattern of response has been reported at the neural level. In PTSD, for example, amygdala hyperreactivity is associated with hypervigilance and intrusive thoughts, whereas amygdala hyporeactivity leads to symptoms of dissociation and disengagement [123].

Another consideration concerns whether these maladaptive responses are triggered by specific events or whether they are chronically expressed. According to the diathesis-stress model of anxiety disorders, functional perturbation of these adaptive mechanisms should be the greatest during periods of anxiety or stress (e.g., during experimental threat). Alternatively, these mechanisms could be chronically and maladaptively engaged, even in the absence of experimental threat.

Step 5: treatment implications

Given the poor predictive validity of animal models, another promising clinical utilization of experimental psychopathology is treatment research. Although there is a dearth of research in this area, the two broad applications of experimental models in humans are to explore the mechanisms through which treatments exert their effect and to screen candidate anxiolytics in proof-of-concept studies.

Treatment mechanisms

The mechanism by which conventional pharmacological treatments, such as the SSRIs and the benzodiazepines, or cognitive–behavioral therapy exert their effect is poorly understood. Experimental psychopathology is being used to clarify mechanisms responsible for the clinical efficacy of current pharmacological or psychological treatments. The results show that the effectiveness of SSRIs may be partly due to their downregulating effect on attentional bias for threat [34, 39]. Studies focused on defensive mechanisms indicate the benzodiazepine alprazolam and the SSRI citalopram, which are used to treat anxiety patients, reduce defense responses to unpredictable shock but not predictable shock in healthy subjects [9, 10]. Interestingly, alcohol, which is used for its anxiety-dampening effect, also reduces defense responses to unpredictable but not predictable shock [125]. As research in humans and rodents points to distinct neurocircuits responsible for the response to predictable and unpredictable threat, these results provide clues as to the structures that may be preferentially implicated in these anxiolytic effects [12].

As expected, given the role of cognitive dysfunction in anxiety disorders, there is also evidence that conventional anxiolytics act on cognitive processes. Studies in healthy volunteers have shown that 7 days of treatment with the benzodiazepine diazepam or with the SSRI citalopram reduces a pattern of attentional vigilance to threat [126, 127]. Because cognitive models suggest that anxiety disorders are associated with attentional biases for threat, these results suggest that the therapeutic action of classical anxiolytics may partly be mediated by normalizing these biases.

Proof of concept

Anxiolytics that act preferentially through the γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) or serotoninergic systems have been the benchmark since the 1950s and 1980s, respectively. To improve the efficacy of anxiolytics, research has sought to discover new compounds that either target these systems or have a novel mode of action, for example, acting on the neuropeptide, glutamate, or endocannabinoid systems [31]. However, despite important breakthroughs in basic science that have led to the development of a multitude of drug anxiolytic candidates, the ability to bring to the marketplace efficient new compounds has not improved. The lack of success of drug development can be traced to the difficulties in selecting among the many candidate anxiolytic compounds, because of costly and time-consuming clinical trials, and the failure of animal models of anxiety to predict clinical efficacy. As long as candidate anxiolytics are safe, there is no reason why they could not be tested in models of anxiety, using healthy individuals to establish whether they are actually anxiolytic.

The predictive validity of a model refers the ability of the model to predict clinical efficacy. However, a model has predictive validity if it can successfully differentiate among effective and ineffective treatments. Animal models have a high level of sensitivity but poor specificity. The hope is that human models can add specificity and help drug sponsors make the “go–no-go” decision about novel compounds and choose appropriate treatment doses for subsequent clinical trials. Given their high cost and long duration, clinical trials are limited in the number of novel compounds that can be tested. New drug development methods, such as experimental models in humans, may facilitate and speed up screening, and improve the predictability and the efficacy of candidate anxiolytics.

Whether this approach will be successful remains to be seen. So far, the few studies that have tested putative anxiolytics in anxiety models in humans have had mixed success, as illustrated by translational research on antagonists of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) receptors. Based on strong preclinical evidence that CRF was a key mediator of stress-related responses, it has been suggested that drugs that target the CRF1 systems could be developed to relieve anxiety (as well as depression and alcoholism) [128]. CRF1 antagonists have proved to be anxiolytic in a wide range of animal models [129], but results in human models have not been as consistent. The CRF1 antagonists GSK876008 reduced startle potentiation in animal models of anxiety [130], but acute treatment with a similar CRF1 antagonist, Verucerfont (GSK561679), did not reduce startle potentiation or subjective feeling of anxiety to unpredictable shock in a double-blind, crossover design in healthy subjects [35]. However, a 7-day treatment with another CRF antagonist (R317573), reduced subjective response but not physiological response to a 7.5% CO2 inhalation challenge in a double-blind, randomized, placebo, and active controlled study [36]. These rather negative results should be considered in the light of clinical data that have shown the CRF1 antagonist to be clinically inefficient to treat generalized anxiety disorder, social anxiety disorders, depression, and comorbid anxiety–alcoholism [131, 132]. It is noteworthy that the same CRF1 antagonist that was tested in the fear-potentiated startle model in humans [35], was also found to have no anxiolytic effect when given chronically in individuals with PTSD [133]. Although negative, these results have a positive side. The fear-potentiated startle model in humans did not produce a false positive: a clinically ineffective drug was ineffective in the human model.

Beside screening putative anxiolytics, human models will be necessary to evaluate non-pharmacologic treatments, such as exercise [134], cognitive–behavioral [135], mindfulness, or neuromodulation [40]. One advantage of experimental models in humans is that their sensitivity and specificity may be improved by targeting pathological mechanisms identified via the experimental psychopathology approach.

Examples of the experimental psychopathology approach to pre-attentive information processing

Cognitive models emphasize the role of deficits in early information processing as a primary and persistent manifestation of pathological anxiety, contributing to symptoms of hypervigilance and downstream malfunction of more complex cognitive operations [92, 136, 137]. Various psychological constructs have been employed to study early information-processing deficits in pathological anxiety, including attentional bias [92], sensory gating [138, 139], and pre-attentive perceptual processing [137, 140,141,142]. This section illustrates how experimental psychopathology can provide clues about neurocircuit dysfunction using two specific examples: 1) perceptual sensitivity and 2) negative biases.

Perceptual sensitivity

Step 1: functional domains and markers of anxiety: mismatch negativity

Effective early detection of environmental change is adaptive, as it provides for rapid orienting to potential threats, driving the organism to adopt cautious behavioral strategies [143]. Early perceptual responding to environmental changes can be examined with oddball stimulus paradigms. These paradigms assess how the organism responds to rare, deviant stimuli. A consistent electroencephalographic response to these unexpected stimuli has been identified and labeled mismatch negativity (MMN). In other words, the MMN is a brain-evoked potential in response to stimuli that deviate (prediction errors) from an established familiar sequence of sensory stimuli (e.g., change in tone frequency) [144]. It is considered a measure of pre-attentive detection because it can be elicited when attention is focused elsewhere [145].

As a measure of sensory perception and auditory discrimination, the MMN provides a window into the state of vigilance of the brain [146, 147]. Consistent with this view, the MMN is abnormally elevated in a number of anxiety-related disorders, including PTSD [140, 141], panic disorder [142], and phobia [148]. These findings support brain’s heightened sensitivity to environmental changes in these patient populations (i.e., hypervigilance). Heightened MMN is also associated with behavioral inhibition, a temperamental vulnerability for later anxiety disorders [149]. These results indicate that the MMN can be used as a proximal measure of anxious hypervigilance to gain insights into the underlying neurocircuit dysfunction.

Step 2: normative mechanisms in healthy subjects

While the neural regions contributing to MMN generation have long been elucidated [150], more recent works relying on dynamic causal modeling (DCM) have provided a mechanistic understanding of how the magnetoencephalographic MMN (MMNm) is generated [151]. The MMNm is generated within a well-established frontotemporal network composed of bilateral sources over the primary and secondary auditory cortex (superior temporal gyrus, STG), and inferior frontal gyri (IFG) [151] (Fig. 2, left). DCM shows that the MMN can be explained by changes in the strength of the connectivity between (extrinsic) and within (intrinsic) these cortical sources (Fig. 2, right) [151]. Forward connections can be conceptualized as bottom-up processes transmitting sensory information from A1 to higher cortical levels and convey prediction errors (MMN). Backward connections represent top–down predictions based on prior sensory experience and explain away prediction errors (deviance detection).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Left. The MMNm arises from bilateral sources over primary, secondary auditory cortex (superior temporal gyrus, STG), and inferior frontal gyri (IFG) [150]. Right. Dynamic causal modeling shows that the MMN results from changes within and among these cortical sources [151]. Forward connections can be conceptualized as bottom-up processes transmitting sensory information from A1 to higher cortical levels and convey prediction errors (MMN). Backward connections represent top–down predictions based on prior sensory experience and explain away prediction errors (deviance detection). In the safe context, the MMN is mediated by changes in both extrinsic feedforward and feedback connectivity, as well as intrinsic connectivity (not shown). Anxiety induced by threat of shock suppresses feedback connectivity. The anxiolytic alprazolam reestablishes feedback connectivity (not shown)

Step 3: studies of the interplay of anxiety and cognition

Consistent with the dimensional view of psychopathology, the MMNm is also increased by experimental anxiety in healthy subjects [104, 105]. The underlying mechanisms responsible for this increase have been recently elucidated via DCM and involve a rebalance of forward and backward connections. Threat of shock enhances postsynaptic gain in the primary auditory cortex and modulation of the feedforward pathway, but attenuates the normal feedback signaling, which results in a failure to attenuate the ascending prediction errors [104]. These results suggest that anxiety-induced hypervigilance results from heightened sensitivity of bottom-up processes and failure of top–down modulation.

Having established via DCM that the changes within the MMNm neurocircuit are responsible for the increased MMNm by anxiety, the next step will be to identify the structure(s) that cause(s) these changes. One potential structure is the amygdala. The amygdala plays a critical role in threat evaluation [152], boosts processing [153], is involved in novelty detection [154, 155], and importantly, is activated by stimulus deviance in a time window that corresponds to IFG activation [105].

Step 4: mechanisms of pathophysiology

The results of experimental psychopathology constrain the search for the perturbation underlying the increased MMN in anxiety pathology. The identification of the normative mechanisms responsible for the increased MMN by anxiety leads to the hypotheses that comparable changes in the balance between feedforward and feedback signaling caused by threat of shock in healthy subjects are also responsible for the increased MMN in anxiety pathology. This hypothesis has not yet been tested.

Step 5: treatment implications

Drugs acting on gamma-aminobutyric acid receptors can downregulate and upregulate hypervigilance [156, 157] and decrease or increase the MMN [158,159,160], respectively. In healthy subjects, the benzodiazepine alprazolam attenuates the threat-modulated MMNm and reestablishes the normal balance between feedforward and feedback signaling [104]. This suggests that benzodiazepines may reduce hypervigilance, partly by dampening early deviance detection. These results suggest that treatment aimed at reducing hypervigilance symptoms in pathological anxiety should target the balance between feedforward and feedback implicated in deviance detection.

Negative biases

Step 1: functional domains and markers of anxiety: attention bias

Attention bias for threatening stimuli has long been associated with pathological anxiety [92]. It reflects the propensity to rapidly detect and react to threat, and can be investigated using a wide range of tests, which reflect different aspects of attention, including the dot probe, the emotional Stroop, and emotional face processing [92, 120, 161]. A face-processing task is the exemplar selected here.

Step 2: normative mechanisms in healthy subjects

Neural models of emotion amplification and emotion regulation suggest several neural signals implicated in attention bias for threat. The amygdala automatically detects salient environmental stimuli [162, 163]. Response flexibility is associated with structures that amplify the threat signals, such as the rodent’s prelimbic cortex or its putative human equivalent, the dACC/dmPFC, and structures that protect goal-directed processing from threat distractors, including the ventromedial PFC (vmPFC), ventrolateral PFC (vlPFC), ACC, and IFG [164,165,166]. It is likely that some of these structures work together for adaptive behavior. Experimental models in humans have helped refine our understanding of functional connectivity among these structures to support bias attention in changing environments.

Step 3: studies of the interplay of anxiety and cognition

Studies in healthy subjects during induced anxiety have highlighted the role of cortical–subcortical connectivity in attention bias for threat. More specifically, in a series of studies using an emotional face identification task, it was first reported that induced anxiety drives attentional bias; subjects show quicker detection of fearful faces and greater defense response (i.e., as measured with startle) upon presentation of fearful faces during threat of shock compared with a safe condition [161, 167]. It was then shown that this heightened attentional bias was associated with increased connectivity between the dACC/dmPFC and the amygdala, providing evidence of an “aversive amplification circuit” that strengthens amygdala response to facilitate threat detection (Fig. 3) [168, 169]. In addition, the strength of this connectivity correlated positively with the behavioral bias index, suggesting that this circuit drives negative bias.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Proposed model of attention bias for threat in anxiety. Increased bidirectional coupling between the dorsal anterior cingulate/dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dACC/dmPFC) and amygdala (amg) promotes threat bias. Coupling is transiently activated during anxiety states in healthy controls, facilitating the detection of threat. However, the coupling is chronically engaged in pathological anxiety, leading to maladaptive threat bias. Lowering serotonin has a similar effect as induced anxiety: it increases dACC/dmPFC–amygdala coupling. By increasing serotonin, SSRIs may reduce attention bias for threat via inhibition of dACC/dmPFC–amygdala coupling

It has been argued that the dACC/dmPFC–amygdala neurocircuit serves to “prime” the amygdala, maintaining the amygdala in a state of readiness. This provides a mechanism by which the amygdala can detect and react rapidly to alerting and potentially dangerous stimuli during sustained anxiety states without maintaining a sustained level of activation [170].

Step 4: mechanisms of pathophysiology

Critically, it was subsequently found that the same dACC/dmPFC–amygdala neurocircuit is overactive in anxiety patients, in the absence of experimental anxiety (i.e., without threat of shock) [98]. In other words, the same neurocircuit responsible for threat bias, which is appropriately engaged and disengaged in healthy subjects by threat and safety, is chronically activated in anxious patients (i.e., in safe environments).

These studies validate the experimental psychopathology approach and further provide evidence that an otherwise self-protective neurocircuit is dysfunctional in anxiety disorders. The positive correlation in healthy individuals between trait anxiety and the strength of the dACC/dmPFC–amygdala coupling [168] suggests that this neurocircuit indexes a vulnerability to anxiety disorders.

Step 5: treatment implications

The dACC/dmPFC–amygdala neurocircuit therefore represents a potential neural target for treatment. Neural markers are more proximal than behavioral markers, and as such, targeting the dACC/dmPFC–amygdala circuit may prove to be a powerful way of developing treatments.

Ideally, one would want to decrease dACC/dmPFC–amygdala coupling [98, 171]. The mechanism by which activity in this neurocircuit can be reduced is not fully understood, but evidence shows that serotonin plays a key role. Serotonin affects affective processing [172, 173] and the first line of psychopharmacological treatment for anxiety disorders, the SSRIs, alters serotonin neurotransmission [174]. In addition, amygdala response to fearful faces is modulated by a serotonin transporter polymorphism [175]. More direct evidence of serotonin involvement in dACC/dmPFC–amygdala coupling comes from findings that, in healthy subjects, reduced serotonin function following depletion of its precursor, tryptophan increases the strength of the connectivity in this circuit [176]. These results suggest that SSRIs may reduce anxiety symptoms by increasing serotonin, which normalizes or reduces the excessive dACC/dmPFC–amygdala coupling responsible for promoting threat bias. This interpretation would be consistent with evidence that serotonin therapeutic effects are mediated by the serotoninergic reduction in attention bias [173, 177]. Figure 3 illustrates this possibility. Given that increased dACC/dmPFC–amygdala coupling promotes threat biases, and reducing serotonin increases dACC/dmPFC–amygdala, SSRIs may reduce threat biases by inhibiting dACC/dmPFC–amygdala coupling. Interestingly, psychological treatments (e.g., cognitive–behavioral therapy) of anxiety disorders, which can be as effective as pharmacological treatment [178], may also work by disengaging this circuit [171].

Future directions

Whether the experimental psychopathology approach will bring a new insight into psychopathology and will help screen novel treatments remains to be seen. Experimental psychopathology is still in its infancy, but recent progresses are encouraging, supporting further development of this approach to fully contribute to clinical research. Below, we briefly review of few issues that need to be addressed to take full advantage of experimental psychopathology:

  1. 1.

    Sex and developmental changes: Anxiety disorders are more prevalent in women [179] and girls experience more anxiety symptoms than boys. Experimental psychopathology can be applied to children and adolescents [180] and can contribute to elucidating the neural basis of this sex difference by characterizing developmental changes.

  2. 2.

    Interindividual differences: Response to anxiety-inducing procedures [181,182,183,184] and cognitive performance [185] vary among individuals, reflecting the influence of temperamental, genetic, or environmental factors. It is likely that the interaction of cognition and experimental anxiety, as well as the treatment effects on these interactions, are similarly affected by interindividual differences. It will be important to identify these factors, since they may critically influence the psychopathological mechanisms and treatment targets. By helping identify the interindividual characteristics that determine treatment effects, experimental psychopathology carries implications for personalized medicine [186].

  3. 3.

    Anxiety-induction procedures: Different classes of environmental threats activate different defense mechanisms. Defense responses to proximal vs. more distal or uncertain threats are distinguishable in terms of behavior, cognition, and neurobiological substrates [187], as well as psychopathology [11]. Similarly, the response to bodily harm differs from that of social threat [188, 189]. Aversive stimuli, such as shocks, have been successfully used in experimental models in humans [66, 67, 190]. However, threat of shock may not be appropriate to model all symptoms of anxiety. For example, a low dose of CO2 (e.g., 7.5%) challenge has been proposed as a model of general anxiety disorder [191]. Behavioral avoidance tasks, where subjects anticipate a feared situation (e.g., anticipation of public speaking in social anxiety) are also a promising tool [192]. For example, healthy controls and patients with social anxiety show distinct emotional responses and cognitive regulation to social threat compared with physical threat [189, 193], and avoidance performance in a virtual elevated plus-maze is associated with the symptom of acrophobia but not social anxiety or trait anxiety [194]. This approach can also be extended to more specific symptoms, such as emotional distraction by worries and intrusive thoughts [195]. Future works will need to examine the commonalities and differences in the effects of different types of anxiety-induction procedures on cognitive and behavioral performance and the underlying neurocircuits [196]. It will be important to determine the extent to which different anxiety-induction procedures model symptoms that are similar or vary across disorders.

  4. 4.

    Drug screening: One exciting potential application of experimental psychopathology is to generate proof-of-concept evidence of the efficacy of novel psychopharmacological and psychological treatments [34, 35, 191]. Currently, determining the efficacy of a drug or psychological intervention requires expensive and time-consuming clinical trials in patients. Experimental psychopathology could provide optimization of this process, leading to a rapid and affordable indication of efficacy at an early stage of treatment development [30]. However, a question for future studies is the extent to which treatments that work on normative responses also work on pathological responses.

Conclusion

Animal models are essential to advance our understanding of anxiety, but their limitations are increasingly recognized. We have proposed a general framework centered on experimental psychopathology to improve research on pathological anxiety. Combining anxiety challenges with tasks that probe clinically relevant psychological or behavioral constructs to identify clinically relevant neurocircuits provides a new approach to translational research on pathological anxiety.

Funding and disclosure

This work was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Mental Health, project number ZIAMH002798 (clinical protocol 02-M-0321 (NCT00047853), 01-M-0185 (NCT00026559)) to CG, grant numbers MR/K024280/1 and MR/K0240817/1 to OJR. OJR has completed consultancy work for IESO digital health and Brainbow and is running an Investigator Initiated Trial with Lundbeck. He holds an MRC Industrial Collaboration Award with Cambridge Cognition. The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.