Introduction

Abnormalities in neural network activity are common across a range of psychiatric disorders and may provide a diagnostic means for early diagnosis1,2. A number of electroencephalographic (EEG) biomarkers are associated with impaired cognitive flexibility, attention, executive functioning, and social behavior3,4. Of particular interest are disturbances in sensory gating and entrainment to 40 Hz auditory stimuli (auditory steady state response; ASSR), which are impaired in patients with schizophrenia and individuals at high risk to transition to psychosis5. Additionally, neural activity abnormalities have been reported in the gamma frequency range (30–80 Hz), either at rest or during cognitive and sensory related task performance1.

Converging evidence suggests chronic NMDA receptor (NMDAR) hypofunction is central to the pathophysiology of schizophrenia and related psychiatric disorders6,7. NMDAR antagonists can transiently recapitulate positive, negative, and cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia in healthy subjects and animal models8,9, including deficits in EEG biomarkers like the ASSR10. These drugs also induce oxidative damage within cortical circuitry11, potentially disrupting excitatory / inhibitory (E/I) balance and causing downstream abnormalities in gamma band oscillations and cognition. Indeed, patients with schizophrenia have well documented impairments in fast-spiking GABAergic interneurons, which may arise from these neurons’ increased susceptibility to oxidative damage12,13. Antipsychotic treatment can counteract NMDA antagonist-induced changes in some EEG biomarkers, though their therapeutic mechanism is somewhat unclear14. Understanding the underlying mechanisms of EEG biomarkers could aid development of new targeted therapies that aim to alleviate cognitive deficits in patients with psychosis15.

Recent genome-wide association studies have identified a number of genetic risk factors for schizophrenia associated with glutamatergic signaling, including the SRR gene which encodes serine racemase16,17. Here we have examined the relationship between EEG biomarker activity and chronic NMDAR hypofunction by utilizing the serine racemase knockout (SRKO) mouse model. These mice lack expression of the enzyme responsible for synthesis of D-serine, a co-agonist at the NMDAR, and consequently exhibit chronic NMDAR hypofunction18. This well-established model exhibits a wide range of schizophrenia-like phenotypes18,19,20,21 and exhibit enhanced oxidative damage and decreased parvalbumin immunoreactivity22. We hypothesized that SRKO mice will demonstrate impairments in translationally-relevant EEG biomarkers that are consistent with deficits associated with schizophrenia. The presence of these deficits may help us understand their underlying mechanisms and relationships to long term NMDAR dysfunction.

Material and methods

Animals

SRKO mice were originally generated as described18. Adult male and female SRKO (−/−) mice and their wild type (WT) littermates were bred in-house from heterozygote SR (+/−) breeding pairs. These mice were maintained on a C57BL/6 background and were used for all experiments. Animals were given access to food and water ad libitum and maintained on a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights-on 7 am). All procedures were performed in accordance with the National Institutes of Health guidelines, ARRIVE guidelines, and in compliance with the animal protocols approved by the VA Boston Healthcare System Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.

Stereotaxic surgery

Adult (postnatal day 70+) mice were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane (5% induction, 1–2% maintenance) and body temperature was maintained with a chemical heating pad throughout the surgery. Epidural EEG screw electrodes (0.10″, Cat No. 8403, Pinnacle Technology Inc., Lawrence, Kansas, USA) were implanted in the skull above the frontal cortex (from bregma: A/P + 1.9 mm, M/L − 1.0 mm) and parietal cortex (A/P − 1.0 mm, M/L + 1.0 mm) with a reference and ground screw implanted above the cerebellum (from lambda, A/P − 1.5 mm, M/L ± 1.3 mm). Animals were given at least 7 days to recover from surgery before any experiments began. EEG/EMG signals were acquired via 3 channel amplifier (Pinnacle Technology), sampled at 2 kHz, and low pass filtered at 200 Hz.

Social task-elicited gamma

We utilized a 3 chamber (Maze Engineers, Boston, MA, USA) task protocol modified from DeVito, et al.20 portrayed in Fig. 1A. SRKO mice and WT littermates were first tethered for EEG recording, and then provided a 15 min habituation period in their home cage. Mice were then placed in a three-chamber arena under low light conditions (14 lx) to measure sociability and social recognition. Before each of the three consecutive stages of this task (see Fig. 1A), mice were provided a five-minute habituation period in the center chamber. During the empty arena habituation, animals had 5 min to explore all three empty chambers. During the 10-min sociability stage, one side chamber contains an unfamiliar, older sex-matched mouse (stranger “1”) in a cage while the opposite chamber contains a similar looking novel object (e.g. a black roll of tape) in a cage. The location of the object and animal were counterbalanced between test animals. During the 10-min social novelty stage the novel object was replaced with an unfamiliar sex-matched mouse (stranger “2”, age-matched to stranger 1) and the test animal could investigate the novel (“2”) and now familiar (“1”) mice.

Figure 1
figure 1

Social Novelty Recognition is Impaired in SRKO mice. (A) Freely behaving SRKO mice (n = 5) and WT littermates (n = 8) were tethered and placed in a three-chamber arena for a five minute habituation, a ten minute sociability stage (novel mouse “1” vs object), and a ten minute social novelty stage (novel mouse “2” vs familiar mouse “1”, see Materials and Methods for detail). During the sociability stage, both WT (white bars) and SRKO mice (black bars) spent a larger percent of time in the chamber containing the novel mouse than the chamber with the novel object (B) suggesting the sociability of SRKO mice is unchanged. There were no between-group differences during the sociability stage for either time spent exploring either chamber (B), number of entries within a 5 cm zone surrounding the novel mouse or object (C), or in the novel mouse/object ratio of these measurements (D), indicating WT and SRKO mice behaved similarly during this phase of the task. During the social recognition stage only WT mice spent more time investigating the novel mouse than the familiar mouse, measured by the percent time in each chamber (E, strong trend) and the number of entries within a 5 cm zone (F). There were significant between-group differences in the novel/familiar mouse ratio for both measurements (G), suggesting that WT animals spent a greater proportion of time investigating novel animals than familiar animals when compared to their SRKO littermates. This suggests SRKO mice demonstrate decreased social novelty recognition or impaired social novelty-related exploration. Stars represent a significant main effect (B) or a significant Holm–Sidak post hoc test following a significant interaction (F) in a two-way ANOVA. Stars in G represent a significant Mann–Whitney U test. In all figures, bar graphs represent mean values ± standard error of the mean (see Supplementary Materials B1 for these values) while boxplots represent the median and 25th–75th percentiles with whiskers that represent minimum and maximum values. Individual values are represented in D and G by hollow squares and triangles. For all experiments the number of stars represents the level of significance (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001).

Post hoc behavioral analysis was performed using video tracking software (Ethovision XT, Noldus) to assess the proportion of time spent in each chamber and the number of entries within 5 cm of each cage. Additionally, this tracking software was used in combination with EEG recording (WinEDR, University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, Scotland) to assess neural activity in freely behaving mice specifically during investigations of the arena, an object, or another mouse (previously described in McNally, et al.23, see Supplementary Material A1). EEG epochs were extracted around the investigation time, and gamma power (25–58 Hz) was examined immediately after investigation onset and normalized to a 4 s “baseline” (0–4 s pre-stimulus). This frequency range was selected based upon our preliminary data and previous studies which identified investigation-induced increases in this range23. Grand averages were taken across all epochs from all animals for each respective genotype after normalization. All animals expressed a left side preference (% time in chamber) during the empty three chamber habituation task, but the number of chamber entries were equal for left and right sides (See Supplementary Materials A1). The influence of baseline side preference on sociability and social novelty should equalize as the side of stimulus presentation was counterbalanced.

Auditory stimulation

Recordings occurred in each animal’s home cage within a sound-attenuated recording chamber (background noise ~ 55 dB). Stimuli were generated by a BK Precision 4052 waveform generator (Yorba Linda, California, USA) using Spike2 software (Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK) or WinWCP (University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, Scotland) and were delivered through speakers adjacent to the home cage. Data was collected with a Micro 1401 mkII interface module (CED, Cambridge, UK) and Pinnacle’s 3 channel amplifier.

Sensory gating

Following a 10-min tethered habituation period, auditory stimuli were presented as pairs of 80 dB 5 kHz tones of 50 ms duration (n = 100 trials) with an inter-trial interval (ITI) of 6 s and a 500 ms inter-stimulus interval (ISI). The average event-related potential (ERP) for the first (S1) and second (S2) stimulus were analyzed as described in Featherstone, et al.24. Briefly, a waveform average of 100 paired-tone presentations was created from raw EEG records. For each ERP, we measured the maximum positive deflection around 20 ms (P20, 15–30 ms after the tone) and the maximum negative deflection around 40 ms (N40, 25–55 ms) following a 100 ms pre-stimulus baseline correction. See Supplementary Material A2A3 for more detail.

ASSR

As the ASSR task yielded negative results, these methods and results appear in the Supplementary Material (A4 and Table S1).

Data and statistical analyses

Data analysis was performed using custom scripts written for Matlab 2016a (Natick, MA, USA). Power spectral density (PSD) analysis was performed using the multi-taper method (social task-elicited gamma, resting state gamma25, Chronux Toolbox, chronux.org) or complex Morlet wavelet analysis (sensory gating, ASSR), as described previously26. See Supplementary Material A2 for more detail on time–frequency spectral analysis. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05. Statistical analysis was run using GraphPad Prism (San Diego, California, USA) and generally entailed a two-way ANOVA or two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with any significant interactions followed up by a Holm–Sidak multiple comparisons test. For comparisons between two groups, unpaired two-tailed Welch’s t tests were used. If the data failed a Kolmogorov–Smirnov test of normality, then a nonparametric analysis was run instead (Mann–Whitney). Repeated measures correlations were calculated in R using the rmcorr package27. Bar graphs represent mean values ± standard error of the mean (reported in Supplementary Material B1) while boxplots represent the 25th–75th percentiles and median with whiskers representing minimum and maximum values.

Results

Social novelty recognition is impaired in SRKO mice

Patients with schizophrenia experience social withdrawal and impaired social and nonsocial recognition memory28, and deficits in social cognition have been linked to altered E/I balance in the cortex29,30. A three-chamber arena was used to assess sociability and social recognition by analyzing the proportion of time spent in each chamber and the number of nose-point entries within 5 cm of each cage (Fig. 1A). During the sociability stage, EEG-tethered mice could investigate a novel mouse or a novel object. Behavioral measures of sociability were not significantly different between WT and SRKO mice. Both WT and SRKO mice spent a larger percent of time in the chamber containing the novel mouse than the chamber with the novel object [Fig. 1B, two-way ANOVA, main effect of object, F(1,22) = 7.543, p = 0.0118], consistent with prior studies20,31. We additionally measured the number of nose point entries in a 5 cm zone surrounding each mouse or object as a complementary analysis. There were no significant differences in the number of entries during the sociability task (Fig. 1C, two-way ANOVA). Using proportions (mouse/object) of the measurements described above, we directly compared the sociability preference between WT and SRKO animals. There were no significant differences between WT and SRKO animals in their preference for a novel mouse over a novel object (Fig. 1D, Welch’s t-test).

During the social novelty stage, mice could freely investigate the same mouse in the same location from the sociability stage (familiar mouse) or a novel mouse where the object had been in the prior stage. WT mice had a strong trend to spend a larger percent time in the novel mouse chamber than the familiar mouse chamber, while SRKO mice had a weak trend in the opposite direction [Fig. 1E, two-way ANOVA, interaction F(1,22) = 8.171, p = 0.0091, (WT) p = 0.0576, (KO) p = 0.0856]. During the social novelty stage, only WT animals had more entries within 5 cm of the novel mouse than the familiar mouse [Fig. 1F, two-way ANOVA, interaction F(1,22) = 5.094, p = 0.0343, WT p = 0.0068]. Together, these results suggest WT but not SRKO animals spent more time investigating the novel mouse than the familiar mouse. The proportion of time spent in the novel/familiar mouse chamber was greater for WT (median = 1.770) than for SRKO animals (median = 0.505, Mann–Whitney U = 6, p = 0.0451, Fig. 1G, left side). Similarly, the number of nose point entries in a 5 cm radius of the novel/familiar mouse was greater for WT (median = 1.971) than for SRKO animals (median = 0.7083, Mann–Whitney U = 5, p = 0.0295, Fig. 1G, right side).

In summary, SRKO animals spent a smaller proportion of time investigating the novel versus familiar mouse compared to controls, indicating SRKO mice have decreased short-term social novelty recognition or impaired social novelty-related exploration. This is supported by another study using a three-chambered approach task in SRKO mice31. Despite reports of hyperactivity in SRKO mice18, the distance traveled and average velocity were not different between WT and SRKO mice during any stage of this task (Figure S1).

Social task-elicited gamma power is impaired in SRKO mice

We additionally recorded social investigation associated gamma power from the frontal cortex during performance of the social novelty task. Here we observed an increase in low gamma activity corresponding with the start of each novel mouse investigation (Fig. 2A,B,D,E). Thus, we focused our analysis on the first second of these investigations using 0.5 s bins. During the sociability task, SRKO mice exhibited a deficit in social task-elicited low gamma power (25–58 Hz) compared to WT littermates from 0.5 to 1 s after the novel mouse investigation began (Fig. 2C, t(372) = 2.318, Holm–Sidak adjusted p = 0.0415). Deficits in elicited gamma also emerged during the social recognition task. During the first second of novel mouse investigation, WT animals had a significantly larger increase in elicited low gamma power (25–58 Hz) compared to SRKO animals (Fig. 2F, 0–0.5 s, t(332) = 2.896, Holm–Sidak adjusted p = 0.0040; 0.5–1 s, t(332) = 3.490, Holm–Sidak adjusted p = 0.0011). Elicited gamma was comparable between genotypes during the object and familiar mouse investigations (Figure S2).

Figure 2
figure 2

Social Investigation-Elicited Gamma Power Is Impaired in SRKO mice. Frontal cortex social investigation-elicited gamma power (25–58 Hz) was recorded during the behavioral task described in Fig. 1. Grand average spectrograms appear in (A,B) and (D,E) (% of baseline power), while the investigation-elicited gamma power normalized to background gamma is graphed in (C) and (F). The dotted line represents the start of the novel mouse investigation (time 0) and the white boxes outline the data that was analyzed in 0.5 s bins. WT mice (n = 8) had a significantly larger increase in gamma power than SRKO animals (n = 5) during 0.5–1 s for the sociability task (C), and 0–1 s for the social novelty task (F). Greater background gamma power (0–4 s prior to novel mouse investigation, percent of total power) was evident in SRKO mice compared to WT littermates during the social novelty task (H) but not the sociability task (G), suggesting an improper signal-to-noise ratio may contribute to the difference in elicited gamma. Furthermore, there were significant inverse correlations such that greater background gamma power was associated with reduced investigation-elicited gamma power for all novel mouse investigations across both trials for WT (I, n = 232 investigations) and SRKO (J, n = 127 investigations) animals. In the repeated measures correlations, each dot represents a single investigation of a novel mouse, and each individual mouse is represented by a unique color. The data in Fig. 1 and 2 suggest SRKO mice have a deficit in social-elicited gamma in response to a novel mouse investigation. Enhanced background gamma in SRKO mice may be a contributing factor to this deficit. Stars represent significance from multiple t-tests with Holm–Sidak correction (C,F) or a Mann–Whitney U test (H).

To determine if an improper signal-to-noise ratio of gamma power may contribute to this difference, we compared the background gamma power (0–4 s pre-investigation, 25–58 Hz, percent of total power) between genotypes during novel mouse investigations. During the sociability stage, there was no difference in background gamma between genotypes (Fig. 2G). However, during the social novelty stage, SRKO mice had significantly greater background gamma power (median = 22.64) than WT littermates (median = 19.24, Fig. 2H, Mann–Whitney U = 1436, p < 0.0001). Furthermore, there were significant inverse correlations between investigation-elicited gamma power and background gamma power for all novel mouse investigations across both trials for WT (Fig. 2I; repeated measures correlation, WT r = − 0.251, p = 1.56e−4) and SRKO animals (Fig. 2J; r = − 0.187, p = 0.0387). Altogether, our data show that SRKO have impaired social task-elicited gamma activity in response to investigating a novel mouse, perhaps corresponding to impaired task performance. Enhanced background gamma in SRKO mice may be a contributing factor to their deficit in elicited gamma. However, this appears to be task specific, as no changes in gamma power were observed during resting state activity in a separate context (Figures S3, S4).

Sensory gating is impaired in SRKO mice

Sensory gating is an auditory processing phenomenon impaired in patients with schizophrenia and associated with hallucinations or delusions5,32. The frontal cortex grand average ERPs generated by paired tones (S1 & S2) were examined (Fig. 3A). Although N40 was larger in S1 than S2 for all animals, only WT animals had significantly larger P20 and P20–N40 amplitudes in S1 than in S2 (two-way RM ANOVA, p < 0.05, Fig. 3B, P20 interaction F(1,17) = 4.646, N40 main effect F(1,17) = 18.33, Fig. 3C, P20–N40 interaction F(1,17) = 5.213, see Supplementary Materials B2). Comparison of S2/S1 and S1–S2 “normalized” ratios revealed impaired sensory gating of P20–N40 amplitude in the frontal cortex of SRKO mice compared to WT littermates [unpaired two-tailed Welch’s t-test, Fig. 3D, S2/S1, t(13.39) = 2.887, p = 0.0124; Fig. 3E, S1–S2, t(15.40) = 2.256, p = 0.039].

Figure 3
figure 3

Sensory Gating is Impaired in SRKO mice. Sensory gating was measured in the frontal cortex EEG of SRKO mice (n = 10) and WT littermates (n = 9) by averaging the evoked response potential (ERP) of 100 repetitions of two identical 5 kHz 50 ms tones (S1 and S2) separated by 500 ms. Frontal cortex grand average ERP’s are superimposed in (A) for WT (left) and SRKO (right) animals. Although N40 was larger in S1 than S2 for all animals, only WT animals had significantly larger P20 and P20–N40 amplitudes in S1 than in S2 (B,C). The P20–N40 amplitude was normalized as a ratio between ERP1 (from S1) and ERP2 (from S2) before comparing between groups. Compared to WT littermates, SRKO mice had a reduced gating response of P20–N40 amplitude in the frontal cortex as evidenced by a larger S2/S1 ratio (D) and an attenuated difference between S1 and S2 P20–N40 amplitudes (E). Stars in (B) and (C) represent a main effect of stimulus in a two-way RM ANOVA (N40) or significance in a Holm–Sidak post hoc test following a significant interaction in a two-way RM ANOVA (P20, P20–N40). Stars in (D) and (E) represent significance in an unpaired two-tailed Welch’s t-test.

Next, we examined whether there was a sensory gating deficit in evoked gamma power (30–80 Hz), as has been reported in patients with schizophrenia and a mouse model of schizophrenia33. As shown in Fig. 4A,B, KO mice also demonstrated impaired sensory gating of frontal cortex gamma power compared to WT [unpaired two-tailed Welch’s t-test, t(10.14) = 2.634, p = 0.0247]. Specifically, ERP2 gamma power was 75% reduced from ERP1 in WT mice, while it was only 42% reduced from ERP1 in SRKO mice. We additionally performed PSD comparisons between genotypes for S1 (ERP1) and S2 (ERP2) across the entire frequency range (0.5–100.5 Hz; Fig. 4C,D). Compared to WT, SRKO animals showed an overall decrease in power during ERP1 (Fig. 4C, 1–1.05 s), but not ERP2 (Fig. 4D, 1.50–1.55 s) [ERP1 main effect of genotype, two-way RM ANOVA, F(1,17) = 4.576, p < 0.0472].

Figure 4
figure 4

Power Spectral Density Abnormalities Occur in SRKO mice During Sensory Gating. We examined whether there was a sensory gating deficit in evoked gamma power (30–80 Hz). The frontal cortex grand average spectrogram (A) demonstrates the change in evoked power during S1 (1–1.05 s) and S2 (1.5–1.55 s) for WT (left) and SRKO (right) mice. The frequency comparisons in parts (B–D) were calculated using these stimulus times. Similar to ERP amplitude, SRKO mice also demonstrated impaired sensory gating of gamma power (41.7% reduced from S1 to S2) compared to WT littermates (74.8% reduced, B). To examine task-evoked power spectral density differences between genotypes during each stimulus, the log-transformed normalized power for 0.5–100.5 Hz was analyzed using 1 Hz bins. Compared to WT littermates, SRKO animals had decreased power across the frequency spectrum during ERP1 (C) but not during ERP2 (D). Stars represent a significant unpaired two-tailed Welch’s t-test. (C) had a main effect of genotype in a two-way RM ANOVA, represented by a blue bar under the significantly different frequencies.

Other biomarkers are neurotypical in SRKO mice

Other translationally-relevant electrophysiological biomarkers of schizophrenia investigated, including ASSR and sleep spindles, were unaltered in SRKO mice (see Supplementary Materials A6, B3, and Table S1S3 for more detail). Elicited and induced (non-phase locked “background”) power and 40 Hz phase locking were unchanged in the ASSR task between WT (n = 11) and SRKO (n = 10) mice (Table S1). There were no substantial changes between WT (n = 14) and SRKO (n = 12) animals for the percent time in each sleep/wake vigilance state, the average bout length, or average bout frequency (Table S2). Sleep spindle characteristics (spindle density, amplitude, median and mean duration, median frequency) were not different between WT (n = 7) and SRKO (n = 6) animals (Table S3). In the parietal and frontal cortices, significant differences in EEG frequency power spectra (< 10 Hz) were found between SRKO mice and WT littermates during wake and sleep states in light and dark periods, although these effects were variable (see Supplementary Figures S3 and S4). Parietal cortex data was not significantly different between genotypes for most measurements (see Supplementary Materials B4 and Figure S5).

Discussion

SRKO mice have been well characterized to exhibit behavioral, brain morphological, and neurochemical abnormalities reminiscent of schizophrenia18,19,20,34,35,36. Thus, we tested whether SRKO mice would phenocopy certain EEG biomarker abnormalities observed in patients with schizophrenia or induced by NMDAR antagonists. Here we observed that SRKO mice exhibit deficits in short-term social recognition that corresponded with impaired investigation-elicited gamma power. Additionally, SRKO mice exhibited sensory gating impairments, in terms of both gamma power and ERP amplitude. However, other biomarkers such as ASSR and sleep spindles were unaffected in SRKO mice. Abnormal gamma power, whether task-associated or background, was a common theme across our study and may provide insight into the mechanisms behind these biomarker deficits.

Deficient task-associated gamma in SRKO mice is associated with behavioral impairments

Proper E/I balance maintains stable yet flexible cortical activity, and a signal-to-noise ratio necessary for normal cortical function30. Deficits in GABAergic interneurons are common in many neuropsychiatric disorders and lead to abnormal gamma band oscillations at rest and during tasks2. This likely disturbs E/I balance through cortical disinhibition, leading to “noisier” circuits, and inefficient information processing30. Altered gamma oscillations in SRKO mice would suggest chronic NMDAR hypofunction disrupts the E/I balance with consequences on information processing. Indeed, SRKO mice showed impaired task-associated gamma power in the frontal cortex during social recognition and sensory gating tasks which corresponded with deficits in task performance. Patients with schizophrenia often have impaired frontal cortex gamma band oscillations, which are associated with parvalbumin interneuron dysfunction and aberrant cognitive and perceptual functions2. Therefore, this deficit might be linked to previously reported reductions in cortical parvalbumin interneuron density in mature adult SRKO mice22. In addition, the impaired frontal cortex gamma power that occurred during novel social interactions, but not familiar mouse nor novel object investigations (Figure S2), could be a biomarker for social cognitive dysfunction that indicates a deficit in socially motivated working memory, attention, memory consolidation or retrieval.

We additionally observed that background (pre-investigation) gamma power was abnormally high in the frontal cortex of SRKO mice during performance of the social novelty task (Fig. 2H), but not during independent measurement of gamma at rest (Figure S3A,B). Interestingly, the default mode network (DMN), which is comprised of a collection of brain regions including the prefrontal cortex, exhibits enhanced gamma activity during resting state behavior and suppressed gamma activity during cognitive tasks37. Patients with schizophrenia have an impaired ability to suppress the DMN, potentially because of an E/I imbalance, which contributes to working memory deficits and other cognitive impairments38. Thus, our findings suggest that SRKO mice may have deficient suppression of background gamma activity during certain behavioral contexts (social investigation) that could result in excess “noise” that disrupts the signal-to-noise ratio and cortical E/I balance, contributing to impaired task performance.

Sensory gating deficits in SRKO mice are similar to schizophrenia

Our sensory gating findings revealed multiple similarities between SRKO mice and patients with schizophrenia. Patients with schizophrenia have a lower S1 P50 amplitude, a comparable or larger S2 P50 amplitude, a larger S2/S1 ratio, and a smaller S1–S2 difference during sensory gating4,39. In our study, only WT animals had significant gating of frontal P20 and P20–N40 amplitude (the mouse analogue of the human P50)40,41. Therefore, SRKO mice had a larger S2/S1 ratio and a smaller S1–S2 difference similar to what is often seen in patients with schizophrenia. Furthermore, SRKO mice had deficits in frontal cortex evoked power across the gamma frequency range during sensory gating consistent with the clinical schizophrenia literature and another a genetic mouse model relevant to schizophrenia33. Normalized beta and gamma power were among frequencies lower in SRKO mice than WT littermates during the period when the ERP1 P20 and N40 occur (0–50 ms). Comparable deficits in beta (20–30 Hz) and gamma power (30–50 Hz) were reported in patients with schizophrenia for the human analogues of these peaks (ERP1 P50 and N100, 0–100 ms), and may be a biomarker of a failed initial sensory registration39.

Although impaired sensory gating is observed in patients with schizophrenia, pharmacologically-induced NMDAR hypofunction does not usually affect this biomarker. Neither acute nor chronic administration of the NMDAR antagonists ketamine or MK-801 significantly impairs sensory gating (S2/S1 ratio) in humans, mice, or rats42,43,44. Furthermore, mice with reduced NMDAR NR1 subunit expression have a normal sensory gating response despite increased P20 and N40 amplitudes45, but another group reported a reduced S2/S1 ratio in these mice46. Altogether, this suggests the sensory gating deficits observed in SRKO mice may not be due to NMDAR hypofunction alone. Low cortical dopamine levels may contribute to a sensory gating deficit since ketamine (which induces dopamine release47) does not induce these deficits43 and most antipsychotics with D2-antagonist properties fail to rescue these deficits48. Mice with reduced alpha-7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors have sensory gating impairments33, and clozapine (which enhances acetylcholine49) and nicotine each rescue sensory gating deficits in patients with schizophrenia48,50. Therefore, reduced cholinergic or cortical dopaminergic activity, as seen in schizophrenia51,52, may contribute to the sensory gating deficits in schizophrenia and SRKO mice.

ASSR and spindles are unaffected in SRKO mice

Synchronization of cortical neuron firing in response to repetitive stimuli is believed to depend critically on E/I balance and represents a process related to cognitive function53. Patients with schizophrenia and rodents administered NMDAR antagonists exhibit abnormalities in their ability to synchronize cortical firing in response to 40 Hz auditory stimuli26,54. However, consistent with recently published findings55, ASSR evoked power was intact in SRKO mice at all measured frequencies (Table S1). Phase locking and background (induced) power were also unchanged at 40 Hz (Table S1). Acute versus chronic NMDAR antagonist treatments have competing effects that may explain our results. In tethered, freely moving rats acute MK-801 altered the intertrial coherence of the 40 Hz ASSR in the primary auditory cortex, but chronic (21 day) MK-801 treatment had no significant effects56. Furthermore, the consequences of ketamine on the 40 Hz ASSR in conscious rats depends largely on the dose used, the degree of NMDAR occupancy, and the amount of time since drug administration57. Therefore, our neurotypical ASSR result in SRKO mice is compatible with chronic NMDAR antagonist pharmacological studies. This contrasts deficient task-evoked gamma power in other biomarkers; however, these results may arise from distinct mechanisms. Indeed, the 40 Hz ASSR deficit in patients with schizophrenia may be due to enhanced background gamma activity26, which our mice do not replicate during this task or at rest (Figures S3, S4).

Region specificity could explain why biomarker deficits were seemingly inconsistent and mainly limited to the frontal cortex. The relative expression of SR, D-serine, and glycine in various brain regions could influence NMDAR signaling and the existence or location of biomarker deficits in SRKO mice. If D-serine and glycine coexist in a brain region, the level of activity can influence which one is used as an NMDAR co-agonist58. SRKO mice had no deficits in the 40 Hz ASSR or sleep spindles, which are biomarkers that require intact thalamocortical circuitry59,60 or normal thalamic NMDAR signaling61,62. Regions like the thalamic reticular nucleus do not express high levels of SR (63; unpublished data) and thus likely have normal NMDAR function in SRKO mice. Furthermore, NMDAR antagonists have region-specific consequences on gamma power. MK-801 enhances gamma activity in the hippocampus and above bregma but not in the primary auditory cortex of conscious, freely-behaving rats44. Conversely, bath application of ketamine onto rat coronal brain slices enhances gamma rhythms (30–50 Hz) in the primary auditory cortex, impairs gamma rhythms in the medial entorhinal cortex, and has no effect on hippocampal slices64. Therefore, gamma power in our parietal and frontal electrodes should not necessarily align in our global mouse model of NMDAR hypofunction. Future studies deleting SR in discrete cortical brain regions will address this confound.

NMDAR hypofunction changes delta and theta power

Several observed changes in PSD aligned with NMDAR hypofunction. Chronic NMDAR antagonist treatment can reduce theta and gamma power for weeks or months after the last drug administration in rodents65,66. This may explain the SRKO deficit in broadband power during sensory gating and the reduction in parietal cortex normalized power around the theta band (4–7 Hz) during resting state and sleep behaviors for the lights-off (active) period (Figure S4). We also found small differences in EEG delta power (1–4 Hz) during spontaneous NREM sleep, a frequency band that has been reported to be dysregulated with schizophrenia and is involved in cognition67,68. NMDAR hypofunction could be involved in certain cognitive impairments observed with schizophrenia through enhanced NREM delta power, as this could potentially interfere with thalamic corollary discharge and information transmission during sleep69. However, the largely neurotypical sleep patterns and sleep spindles found in SRKO mice suggest that differences in sleep architecture between genotypes contributed minimally to our cognitive function findings.

Limitations

The social task-elicited gamma experiment had a smaller sample size compared to our other experiments (see Figure Legends and Supplemental Materials). However, social task-elicited gamma power was normalized to background gamma which minimizes inter-mouse variability, and our discovery of lower elicited gamma in SRKO mice during novel mouse investigations was replicated across two trials (sociability and social novelty) suggesting a large effect size. This is supported by clinical and preclinical literature that finds schizophrenia-associated changes in stimulus-evoked gamma are larger and more consistent than changes in resting state gamma26,70.

Conclusions

In conclusion, the SRKO mouse model mimics a subset of EEG and behavioral phenotypes associated with schizophrenia and chronic NMDAR antagonist treatment. These novel biomarker deficits compliment SRKO literature reporting changes similar to positive, negative, and cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia18,19,20,21,55. However, other clinically relevant biomarkers that are usually impaired by NMDAR antagonists or disrupted in patients with schizophrenia were not deficient in SRKO mice. These include the auditory steady-state response, sleep spindles, and state-specific power spectral density. Although they do not fully recapitulate symptoms of schizophrenia, these mice may be useful for modeling patients with chronic schizophrenia more accurately than pharmacologic models in specific domains71 including sensory gating and resting state gamma70. Future studies of SRKO mice can confirm whether (1) the abnormal biomarker phenotypes persist among antipsychotic or D-serine treatment, (2) glycine-related compensatory responses are occurring, (3) thalamocortical circuitry is intact, or (4) abnormalities in dopamine levels, cholinergic signaling, or parvalbumin-containing neurons exist in the neocortex. Recent work indicates SRKO mice have reduced inhibitory tone in hippocampal networks which disrupts neural synchrony and the E/I balance72,73. This, along with our gamma band findings support the idea of an E/I imbalance manifested as an aberrant signal-to-noise ratio impairing cognition and information processing. This deficit may be tied to impaired task-dependent alteration in functional connectivity and impaired suppression of the DMN. Understanding the mechanisms behind these biomarkers could lead to personalized early interventions that prevent the transition to psychosis.

Supplementary Material accompanies this paper. Supplementary citations include74,75.