This page has been archived and is no longer updated

 

Evidence for Meat-Eating by Early Humans

By: Briana Pobiner (Human Origins Program, Smithsonian Institution) © 2013 Nature Education 
Citation: Pobiner, B. (2013) Evidence for Meat-Eating by Early Humans. Nature Education Knowledge 4(6):1
Email
The first major evolutionary change in the human diet was the incorporation of meat and marrow from large animals, which occurred by at least 2.6 million years ago.
Aa Aa Aa

 

Pobiner banner

Eating Meat and Marrow

The diet of the earliest hominins was probably somewhat similar to the diet of modern chimpanzees: omnivorous, including large quantities of fruit, leaves, flowers, bark, insects and meat (e.g., Andrews & Martin 1991; Milton 1999; Watts 2008). Tooth morphology and dental microwear studies suggest that the diet of some hominins may have included hard food items such as seeds and nuts, and underground storage organs (USOs) such as roots and tubers (Jolly 1970; Peters & O'Brien 1981; Teaford & Ungar 2000; Luca et al. 2010). By at least 2.6 million years ago, a remarkable expansion in this diet started to occur; some hominins began incorporating meat and marrow from small to very large animals into their diet. Let's explore the evidence for this dramatic shift using the 5 "W" questions: When, Where, Who, What, Why (and How).

When and where did hominin carnivory first occur?

The strongest evidence for meat and marrow eating are butchery marks found on bones. Slicing meat off a bone with a sharp-edged tool can leave cut marks (Figure 1). Pounding a bone with a large stone to break it open and extract the marrow inside can leave percussion marks. Cut and percussion marks, which together are called butchery marks, may be the result of skinning, disarticulation, and bone breakage for dietary and non-dietary reasons (Blumenschine & Pobiner 2006). Scientists began to recognize these butchery marks on Early Stone Age fossil assemblages in the 1980s (e.g., Bunn 1981; Potts & Shipman 1981; Blumenschine & Selvaggio 1988). Experimental and prehistoric evidence for human chewing on bones has only recently begun to be explored (e.g., Landt 2007; Delaney-Rivera et al. 2009; Fernandez-Jalvo and Andrews 2011; Pickering et al. 2013).

(a) 1.5 million-year-old fossil antelope lower leg bone (metapodial) from Koobi Fora, Kenya, bearing cut marks; (b) close-up of these cutmarks.
Figure 1
(a) 1.5 million-year-old fossil antelope lower leg bone (metapodial)
from Koobi Fora, Kenya, bearing cut marks; (b) close-up of these
cutmarks.
© 2013 Nature Education Courtesy of Briana Pobiner. All rights reserved. View Terms of Use

Only those fossilized bones with butchery marks can confidently be tied to hominin diet (Blumenschine & Pobiner 2006). The earliest well-accepted evidence for this novel dietary behavior comes from about 2.6 Ma at the site of Gona, Ethiopia (Domínguez-Rodrigo et al. 2005). Probably not coincidentally, it's also around this time that we start to see the first evidence of archaeologically visible accumulations of stone tools (Semaw et al. 2003). There may be evidence of hominin-butchered bones at 3.4 Ma at Dikika, Ethiopia (McPherron et al. 2010), where Australopithecus afarensis remains have been found, but this evidence consists of only a few bone specimens and has been disputed (Domínguez-Rodrigo et al. 2010). The earliest well documented evidence of persistent hominin carnivory from in situ excavated fossil fauna occurring in association with large concentrations of stone tools is at about 2.0 Ma at Kanjera, Kenya (Ferraro et al. 2013). In addition to terrestrial animals, evidence from one site at Koobi Fora shows that hominins began to incorporate aquatic foods like turtles, crocodiles, and fish into their diets by about 1.95 Ma (Braun et al. 2010). Multiple localities at Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania, dating to 1.8 Ma also show evidence of in situ butchered mammal remains, ranging in size from hedgehogs to elephants; these are also associated with large numbers of stone tools (Domínguez-Rodrigo et al. 2007; Blumenschine & Pobiner 2006 and references therein). Three sites at Koobi Fora, Kenya, preserve evidence of several butchered mammals from about 1.5 Ma but are not found in association with any stone tools (Pobiner et al. 2008). Perhaps this signals a shift toward intentional specialization of activities, such as animal butchery and stone tool making, in different areas across the landscape.

Who was eating this meat and marrow?

Currently, there is fossil evidence for at least three species of hominins occurring at around 2.6-2.5 Ma: Australopithecus africanus, Australopithecus garhi, and Paranthropus aethiopicus; H. habilis was established by around 2.4-2.3 Ma (Figure 2). There are no butchered bones (or stone tools) found at stratigraphic levels associated with A. africanus or P. aethiopicus, so those taxa are less likely to be our perpetrators. While butchered bones have been found near A. garhi fossils (de Heinzelin et al. 1999), it's only in the Homo lineage, especially in Homo erectus, that we see biological features often linked to meat-eating, such as a decrease in tooth and gut size and an increase in body and brain size (e.g., McHenry 1992; Aiello and Wheeler 1995; Antón 2003; Braun et al. 2010).

Photograph of a cast of KNM-ER 1813, a 1.9 million-year-old <i>Homo habilis</i> skull from Koobi Fora, Kenya.
Figure 2: Photograph of a cast of KNM-ER 1813, a 1.9 million-year-old Homo habilis skull from Koobi Fora, Kenya.
This is one of the most complete skulls of this species.
© 2013 Nature Education Courtesy of Chip Clark/Smithsonian Institution. All rights reserved. View Terms of Use

What's so unique about this dietary strategy?

The carnivory of hominins is unique among primates in three ways: (1) use of flaked stone tools to access animal resources; (2) acquisition of resources from animals much larger than the hominins themselves (Figure 3); and (3) procurement of animal resources by scavenging. Chimpanzees, our closest living relatives, routinely hunt, capture by hand, and eat meat from colobus or other smaller monkeys (e.g. Mitani and Watts 2001), but meat is a small proportion of their diet and they rarely scavenge (Watts 2008), most likely because they cannot efficiently digest carrion (Ragir et al. 2000). How this novel source of food was first recognized by hominins remains unknown. Hominins would likely not have been able to directly exploit grass as grassland expanded habitats across Africa - (though see Sponheimer et al. 2013) but an increase in large (grazing) animal resources would have been useful for any species that could procure and digest them (Plummer 2004). This shift marks an encroachment of a primate onto the larger carnivore guild, which would have challenged hominins with entirely new selective pressures (Brantingham 1999; Pobiner & Blumenschine 2003; Werdelin & Lewis 2005).

A reconstruction of an elephant butchery by <i>Homo erectus</i> nearly 1 million years ago at Olorgesailie, Kenya.
Figure 3
A reconstruction of an elephant butchery by Homo erectus nearly 1 million years ago at Olorgesailie, Kenya.
© 2013 Nature Education Courtesy of Karen Carr/Smithsonian Institution. All rights reserved. View Terms of Use

Why did hominins start eating more meat and marrow?

"Why" questions are notoriously difficult to answer about the past, but we can examine some of the benefits that meat and marrow provide. Meat and marrow are calorie-dense resources with essential amino acids and micronutrients (Milton 1999), and aquatic fauna offer resources rich in nutrients needed for brain growth (e.g., Broadhurst et al. 2002). Increasing the consumption of animal foods could have allowed hominins to increase their body size without losing mobility, agility, or sociality (Milton 1999). But what was the frequency and quantity of nutrients obtained by hominins from animal tissues versus other foods? Hominins at sites FLK 22 and FLKN 1-2, Olduvai Gorge, broke long bones of small to medium-large mammals in direct proportion to their estimated gross caloric yield from marrow fat (Blumenschine & Madrigal 1993 - but see Bunn et al. 2010 for a different interpretation of hominin behavior at FLKN 1-2). Long bone abundance of medium-large mammals at FLK 22 is also correlated significantly and positively to the net yield of marrow bones (Blumenschine & Madrigal 2000). Optimal foraging theory dictates that foods in the optimal diet set are expected to be consumed whenever encountered; the carcass encounter rate is dependent on a variety of ecological variables (Blumenschine & Pobiner 2006). This indicates that by at least 1.8 million years ago, carcass-processing decisions may have taken into account the energy yield of a variety of foods. Assuming opportunistic encounters with carcasses, these net yields are comparable to, or higher than, those for most, if not all, non-mammal food items harvested by tropical hunter gatherers (Blumenschine & Pobiner 2006 and references therein).

How did early humans obtain and utilize this meat and marrow?

The earliest evidence for hunting technology in the form ofhafted spear points, currently dates back to about 500,000 years ago (Wilkins et al. 2012); complex projectile weapons only appear at 71,000 years ago (Brown et al 2012). Persistence hunting has been suggested as a mode of hunting that would have been possible without advanced technology, but it's not clear how we would recognize this behavior in the fossil or archaeological record. The earliest firm evidence for the controlled use of fire at hearths in the form of burned seeds, wood, and flint, likely related to cooking, dates back to about 790,000 years ago (Goren-Inbar et al. 2004). Evidence for earlier traces of fire in eastern and southern Africa associated with hominins at Koobi Fora, Chesowanja, and Swartkrans (e.g., Gowlett et al. 1981; Brain & Sillen 1988; Bellomo 1994) consists primarily of sediment discoloration and is not widely accepted (James 1989; Goren-Inbar et al. 2004). While modern human gut proportions and size are unique among great apes (Milton 1999), and studies have found signatures of selection in genes in modern humans that may have played a role in adaptations to dietary changes (Babbitt et al. 2011), it's unclear exactly when these anatomical and physiological changes that facilitated meat and marrow-eating took place.

Many zooarchaeologists who study Early Stone Age faunal assemblages think it's likely that at least some animal carcasses that were butchered by hominins, especially the larger ones, were obtained by scavenging. Soon after butchery marks were recognized on Early Stone Age fossils, articles on the ‘hunting or scavenging debate' in which hunting is implicitly viewed as behaviorally superior to and more ‘modern' than scavenging increasingly proliferated in the literature, especially centered around interpretations of the FLK 22 Zinjanthropus site at Olduvai Gorge (e.g. Binford 1981; Bunn 1981; Bunn 1986; Shipman 1986; Blumenschine 1988, 1995; Binford 1988; Bunn and Kroll 1986, 1988; Bunn and Ezzo 1993; Capaldo 1997; Domínguez-Rodrigo 1997; Dominguez-Rodrigo et al. 2007). Meanwhile, a series of actualistic studies of resource availability from scavenged carcasses was initiated (Blumenschine 1986, 1987; Cavallo and Blumenschine 1989; Selvaggio 1994; Capaldo 1995, Domínguez-Rodrigo 1999; Pobiner 2007), though some still viewed meat as a marginal food resource (e.g., Speth 1989). A history of this debate is beyond the scope of this paper (but see Bunn 1991; Domínguez-Rodrigo 2002; Domínguez-Rodrigo & Pickering 2003; and Plummer 2004 for reviews); it is not likely that these modes of carcass procurement - hunting and scavenging (whether passive scavenging or active/confrontational scavenging) - were mutually exclusive behaviors, but were both employed depending on a variety of behavioral and ecological variables (e.g., available hominins in the group for carcass procurement, butchery, and transport; prey size, age, and species; habitat, other available food resources, and presence of other predators). Experimental models of the frequency and location of cut, percussion, and tooth marks (e.g., Blumenschine 1988) are most often used to inform us about the timing of access (early access vs. late access), and accumulator(s) that contributed to a zooarchaeological assemblage (e.g., Blumenschine 1995; Egeland et al. 2004).

Some unresolved questions in this area of research are:

1. How important were animal resources to hominins (versus plants and other non-animal resources), and how did this importance vary by hominin species, time period, habitat, or other variables?

2. How does the amount of meat and marrow available for scavenging in modern ecosystems vary with the size of prey (e.g., Blumenschine 1987; Pobiner 2007), the species of prey, predator species, predator group size, and ecological variables such as season and habitat? Would any of these variables affect frequency and location of butchery marks, and if so, how (e.g., Pobiner and Braun 2005)?

3. How can we evaluate whether confrontational scavenging or passive scavenging took place at any one site? What if more than one mode of carcass procurement took place? How did the acquisition of carcasses vary with different ecological affordances at different sites? How does the mode of carcass procurement relate to the timing of hominin access to animal resources (early access or late access)?

Glossary

hominin: Refers to the human evolutionary group of species, including fossil and modern. This word comes from Hominini, a formal biological term in between the level of genus (e.g., Homo, Australopithecus) and the level of family (Hominidae)

carnivory: Obtaining foods from animals.

in situ: (Latin) meaning 'in the place.' In prehistoric studies, in situ refers to an artifact or fossil that occurs in the location where it was deposited. In situ materials are securely situated in a sediment layer, which allows archaeologists to date them and/or give them better context by studying other artifacts, fossils, or sediments that have been are found nearby in the same layer.

fauna: Animals, or pertaining to animals (such as faunal remains).

persistence hunting: A hunting technique in which the hunters use running, walking, and tracking to pursue their prey to the point of prey exhaustion.

Early Stone Age: A time period lasting from about 2.6 million to between 400,000 and 250,000 years ago that includes stone tools traditions called Oldowan and Acheulean. The Early Stone Age in Africa is roughly equivalent to what is called the Lower Paleolithic in Europe and Asia.

actualistic: A method of inferring the nature of past events by analogy with processes observable and in action in the present.

passive scavenging: Scavenging from an animal carcass that was killed by another predator, or that died of natural causes. Can yield a variety of amounts of different carcass resources (e.g. meat, marrow, brains) depending on whether another predator(s) had access to that carcass first and the sizes and species of the predator(s) and prey carcass.

active or confrontational scavenging: Scavenging from a carcass that involves confronting or chasing a predator in order to obtain resources from that carcass. Can yield a variety of amounts of different carcass resources (meat, marrow, brains) depending on whether another predator(s) had access to that carcass first and the sizes and species of other predator(s) and prey carcass. Often (incorrectly) assumed to yield more resources than passive scavenging.

early access: Obtaining resources from a carcass early in the carcass consumption sequence (usually first), whether by hunting or scavenging.

late access: Obtaining resources from a carcass later in the carcass consumption sequence (not first). Late access predators can obtain a variety of amounts of different carcass resources (meat, marrow, brains) depending on the size and species of other predator(s) had access to that carcass first and size of the prey carcass.

References and Recommended Reading


Aiello, L. C., & Wheeler, P. The expensive-tissue hypothe­sis: The brain and digestive system in human and primate evolu­tion. Current Anthropology 36, 199-221 (1995).

Andrews, P. & Martin, L. Hominoid dietary evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 334, 199-209 (1991).

Antón, S.C. Natural history of Homo erectus. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 46, 126-170 (2003).

Babbitt, C. C. et al. Genomic signatures of diet-related shifts during human origins. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 278, 961-969 (2011).

Bellomo, R. V. Methods of determining early hominid behavioral activities associated with the controlled use of fire at FxJj 20 Main, Koobi Fora. Journal of Human Evolution 27, 173-195 (1994).

Binford, L. R. Bones: Ancient Men and Modern Myths. Academic Press, New York (1981).

Binford, L. R. Fact and fiction about the Zinajnthropus floor: data, arguments, and interpretations. Current Anthropology 29, 123-135 (1988).

Blumenschine, R. J. Carcass consumption sequences and the archaeological distinction of hunting and scavenging. Journal of Human Evolution 15, 639-659 (1986).

Blumenschine, R. J. Characteristics of an early hominid scavenging niche. Current Anthropology 28, 383-407 (1987).

Blumenschine, R. J. An experimental model of the timing of hominid and carnivore influence on archaeological bone assemblages. Journal of Archaeological Science 15, 483-502 (1988).

Blumenschine, R. J. Percussion marks, tooth marks, and experimental determinations of the timing of hominid and carnivore access to long bones at FLK Zinjanthropus, Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania. Journal of Human Evolution 29, 21-51 (1995).

Blumenschine, R. J. & Madrigal, T .C. Variability in long-bone marrow yields of East African ungulates and its zooarchaeological implications. Journal of Archaeological Science 20, 555-587 (1993).

Blumenschine, R. J. & Pobiner, B. L. Zooarchaeology and the ecology of Oldowan hominin carnivory. In Ungar, P. (Ed.), Early Hominin Diets: The Known, the Unknown, and the Unknowable. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 167-190 (2006).

Blumenschine, R. J. & Selvaggio, M. M., Percussion marks on bone surfaces as a new diagnostic of hominid behavior. Nature 333, 763-765 (1988).

Blumenschine, R. J. et al. Vertebrate taphonomic perspectives on Oldowan hominin land-use in the Plio-Pleistocene Olduvai basin, Tanzania. In Breathing Life into Fossils: Taphonomic Studies in Honor of C. K. (Bob) Brain. eds. Pickering, T. R., Toth, N. & Schick, K. (Indiana: Stone Age Institute Press, 2007). 161-180.

Brain, C. K. & Sillen, A. Evidence from the Swartkrans cave for the earliest use of fire. Nature 336, 464-466 (1988).

Brantingham, P. J. Hominid-Carnivore coevolution and invasion of the predatory guild. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 17, 327-353 (1998).

Broadhurst, C. L. et al. Brain-specific lipids from marine, lacustrine, or terrestrial food resources: potential impact on early African Homo sapiens. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part B: Biochemistry and Molecular Biology 131(4), 653-673.

Braun, D. R. et al. Early hominin diet included diverse terrestrial and aquatic animals 1.95 Ma in East Turkana, Kenya. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 107, 10002-10007 (2010).

Brown, K. S. et al. An early and enduring advanced technology originating 71,000 years ago in South Africa. Nature 491, 590-593.

Bunn, H. T. Archaeological evidence for meat-eating by Plio-Pleistocene hominids from Koobi Fora and Olduvai Gorge. Nature 291, 547-577 (1981).

Bunn, H. T. Patterns of skeletal representation and hominid subsistence activities at Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania and Koobi Fora, Kenya. Journal of Human Evolution 15, 673-690 (1986).

Bunn, H. T. A taphonomic perspective on the archaeology of human origins. Annual Review of Anthropology 20, 433-467 (1991).

Bunn, H. T. & Ezzo, J. A. Hunting and scavenging by Plio-Pleistocene hominids: nutritional constraints, archaeological patterns and behavioural implications. Journal of Archaeological Science 20, 365-398 (1993).

Bunn, H. T. & Kroll, E. M. Fact and fiction about the Zinajnthropus floor: data, arguments, and interpretations. Current Anthropology 29, 135-149 (1988).

Bunn, H T. et al. Was FLK North levels 1-2 a classic "living floor" of Oldowan hominins or a taphonomically complex palimpsest dominated by large carnivore feeding behavior? Quaternary Research 74(3), 355-362.

Capaldo, S. D. Inferring hominid and carnivore behavior from dual-patterned archaeofaunal assemblages. Ph.D. dissertation, Rutgers University (1995).

Capaldo, S. D. Experimental determinations of carcass processing by Plio-Pleistocene hominids and carnivores at FLK 22 (Zinjanthropus), Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania. Journal of Human Evolution 33, 555-597 (1997).

Cavallo, J. A. & Blumenschine R. J. Tree-stored leopard kills: expanding the hominid scavenging niche. Journal of Human Evolution 18, 393-399 (1989).

Delaney-Rivera, C. et al. Pits and pitfalls: taxonomic variability and patterning in tooth mark dimensions. Journal of Archaeological Science 36, 2597-2608 (2009).

de Heinzelin, J. et al. E. Environment and behavior of 2.5-million-year-old Bouri hominids. Science 284, 625-629 (1999).

Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. Meat-eating by early hominids at the FLK 22 Zinjanthropus site, Olduvai Gorge (Tanzania): an experimental approach using cut-mark data. Journal of Human Evolution 33, 669-690 (1997).

Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. Flesh availability and bone modifications in carcasses consumed by lions: palaoecological relevance in hominid foraging patterns. Palaeogeography, Palaeoclimatology, and Palaeoecology 149, 373-388 (1999).

Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. Hunting and scavenging by early humans: the state of the debate. Journal of World Prehistory 16, 1-54 (2002).

Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. & Pickering, T. R. Early hominid hunting and scavenging: a zooarchaeological review. Evolutionary Anthropology 12, 275-292 (2003).

Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. et al.Configurational approach to identifying the earliest hominin butchers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 107, 20929-20934 (2010).

Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. et al. Cutmarked bones from Pliocene archaeological sites at Gona, Afar, Ethiopia: Implications for the functions of the world's oldest stone tools. Journal of Human Evolution 48, 109-121 (2005).

Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. et al. (Eds.) Deconstructing Olduvai: A Taphonomic Study of the Bed I Sites. Springer, Netherlands (2007).

Egeland, C. P. et al. Disentangling Early Stone Age palimpsests: determining the functional independence of hominid- and carnivore-derived portions of archaeofaunas. Journal of Human Evolution 47, 343-357 (2004).

Fernández-Jalvo, Y. & Andrews, P. When humans chew bones. Journal of Human Evolution 60, 117-123 (2011).

Ferraro et al. Earliest archaeological evidence of persistent hominin carnivory. PLoS ONE 8, e62174 (2013).

Goren-Inbar, N. et al. Evidence of hominin control of fire at Gesher Benot Ya‘aqov, Israel. Science 304, 725-727 (2004).

Gowlett, J. A. J. et al. Early archaeological sites, hominid remains and traces of fire from Chesowanja, Kenya Nature 294, 125-129 (1981).

James, S. R. Hominid use of fire in the lower and middle Pleistocene: a review of the evidence. Current Anthropology 30, 1-26 (1989).

Jolly, C. J. The seed-eaters: a new model of hominid differentiation based on a baboon analogy. Man 5, 1-26 (1970).

Landt, M. J. Tooth marks and human consumption: ethnoarchaeological mastication research among foragers of the Central African Republic. Journal of Archaeological Science 34, 1629-1640 (2007).

Luca, F., Perry, G. H. & Di Rienzo, A. Evolutionary adaptations to dietary changes. Annual Review of Nutrition 30, 291-314 (2010).

McHenry, H. M. Body size and proportions in early hominids. American Journal of Physical Anthropoogy 87, 407-431 (1992).

McPherron, S. P. et al. Evidence for stone-tool-assisted consumption of animal tissues before 3.39 million years ago at Dikika, Ethiopia Nature 466, 857-860 (2010).

Milton, K. A hypothesis to explain the role of meat-eating in human evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology 8, 11-21 (1999).

Mitani, J. C. & Watts, D. P. Why do chimpanzees hunt and share meat? Animal Behavior 61, 915-924 (2001).

Peters, C. R. & O'Brian, E. M. The early hominid plant-food niche: insights from an analysis of plant exploitation by Homo, Pan, and Papio in eastern and southern Africa. Current Anthropology 22, 127-140 (1981).

Pickering, T. R. et al. Taphonomy of ungulate ribs and the consumption of meat and bone by 1.2-million-year-old hominins at Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania. Journal of Archaeological Science 40, 1295-1309 (2013).

Pobiner, B. Hominin-Carnivore Interactions: Evidence from Modern Carnivore Bone Modification and Early Pleistocene Archaeofaunas (Koobi Fora, Kenya; Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania). Ph.D. Dissertation, Rutgers University (2007).

Pobiner, B. L. & Blumenschine, R. J. A taphonomic perspective on the Oldowan hominid encroachment on the carnivoran paleoguild. Journal of Taphonomy 1, 115-141 (2003).

Pobiner, B. L. & Braun, D. R. Strengthening the inferential link between cutmark frequency data and Oldowan hominid behavior: Results from modern butchery experiments. Journal of Taphonomy 3, 107-119 (2005).

Pobiner, B. L. et al.,New evidence for hominin carcass processing strategies at 1.5 Ma, Koobi Fora, Kenya. Journal of Human Evolution 55, 103-130. (2008).

Potts, R. & Shipman, P. Cutmarks made by stone tools on bones from Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania. Nature 291, 577-580 (1981).

Ragir, S. et al. Gut morphology and the avoidance of carrion among chimpanzees, baboons, and early hominids. Journal of Anthropological Research 56, 477-512 (2000).

Selvaggio, M. M. Evidence from carnivore tooth marks and stone-tool-butchery marks for scavenging by hominids at FLK Zinjanthropus, Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania. Ph.D. dissertation, Rutgers University (1994).

Semaw et al. 2.6-Million-year-old stone tools and associated bones from OGS-6 and OGS-7, Gona, Afar, Ethiopia. Journal of Human Evolution 45, 169-177 (2003).

Shipman, P. Scavenging or hunting in early hominids: theoretical framework and tests. American Anthropologist 88, 27-43 (1986).

Speth, J. D. Early hominid hunting and scavenging: the role of meat as an energy source. Journal of Human Evolution 18, 329-343 (1989).

Sponheimer et al. Isotopic evidence of early hominin diets. PNAS doi:10.1073/pnas.1222579110 (2013).

Teaford, M. F. & Ungar, P. S. Diet and the evolution of the earliest human ancestors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 97, 13506-13511 (2000).

Watts, D. P. Scavenging by chimpanzees at Ngogo and the relevance of chimpanzee scavenging to early hominin behavioral ecology. Journal of Human Evolution 54, 125-133 (2008).

Werdelin, L. & Lewis, M. Plio-Pleistocene Carnivora of eastern Africa: species richness and turnover patterns. Zoological Journal of the Linnean Society 144, 121-144 (2005).

Wilkins et al. Evidence for early hafted hunting technology. Science 338, 942-946.

Email

Flag Inappropriate

This content is currently under construction.

Connect
Connect Send a message


Nature Education Home

Evolution

Visual Browse

Close