Introduction

With societal progress and economic development as well as improvements in infrastructure, people's lifestyles and ideas are changing. The acceleration of the pace of life and the intensification of competition have caused People living in large cities suffer from various pressures due to their studies, work, and daily lives1, and Rates of depression, suboptimal health, chronic diseases, and cardiovascular diseases are rising among urban populations2. Research indicates that people have a strong preference for natural environments over urbanized, developed areas3, and for people living in urban environments, urban green spaces are an important component of well-being but are often in short supply. Furthermore, the availability of urban green spaces is a key factor influencing the well-being and quality of life of city dwellers. Urban green spaces can positively impact well-being and health in numerous ways4. Increased activity levels due to contact with nature can provide benefits5.

Landscape perception and experience are primarily understood through the study of human–environment interactions and the objective features of the environment. and with the objective environment, Moreover, insights from these studies are integrated with landscape aesthetics theories to examine how people's satisfaction and preferences are shaped by their environmental experiences6. Landscape perception and experience have been a continuous focus of research due to their importance in understanding human–environment relationships.7. Related studies have explored landscape assessment, planning and design from different perspectives, as this topic has become a vast field with various theoretical directions and approaches8. With global urbanisation, understanding public perceptions of and preferences for urban green spaces has become particularly indispensable for promoting human well-being and quality of life through human-centred design9,10. Although some of the findings from research on landscape perception and experience have been directly implemented in practice, there are questions about the reliability of the strategies adopted by users, planners or practitioners, which may lead to a mismatch between the public demand for green spaces and the actual design of a city11. Landscape perception and experience are strongly dependent on people's visual perception, but various known and unknown factors associated with site perception may influence these strategies. Therefore, comparing the effects of site perception, e.g., site characteristics and time of visit (season), on landscape perception and experience remains challenging.

Influence of visual experience on environmental perception

People's perception of the outside world is mainly obtained through sensory stimulation, and vision, hearing, smell, and touch are important means for perceiving information12. The perception of the environment is particularly important, and people have an innate preference for nature; even short periods of playing or walking in large urban parks or urban woodland landscapes can enhance people's positive emotions13. Some scholars have found that viewing woodland and single-layer grassland landscapes has a more significant effect on people's physical and mental health than viewing other types of landscapes14. In a visual assessment of urban recreational landscapes, Polat et al. reported that the type of green space (e.g., water or plants) impacts visual quality, and it has also been mentioned that a lack of variation in the plant community type has a negative impact on visual quality15.

Walking in and seeing natural environments can enhance positive emotions and relieve stress16,17. Short stays in urban green spaces increase positive emotions and significantly relieve negative emotions that originate in developed environments18. Current research methods on landscape experience usually emphasise the public's enjoyment of the landscape in terms of visual sensory stimulation, and diverse methods, including photographic viewing, video viewing, and virtual reality (VR) technologies, have emerged for experiencing landscape types19. With the development of image technology, an increasing number of researchers have studied the convenience, ease of manipulation and ability to modify images while using photo, video, and VR technologies to present visual landscape experiences. Due to limitations associated with photographic and perceptual experiences, participants are unable to fully experience landscape features and capture feedback through multiple sensory responses, and the effectiveness of the landscape experience may be affected. Being at a location enables more direct and effective landscape perception, allowing individuals to experience the landscape more realistically and effectively20.

Impact of seasonal changes on environmental perceptions

The natural environment consists of a variety of landscape elements, and the combination and layout of these different elements form diverse landscape environments. Green space is an important part of landscape environments, and these elements are associated with various vegetation types. The incorporation of plant community types in landscapes is important21. A combination of plant community types can beautify the environment and create space for recreation. People recognise that health can be improved by exposure to greenery; plant community types play a role in beautifying the environment and creating space22, and there is an increasing demand for space23. When people discovered the importance of green spaces for physical and mental health, an increasing number of people began to desire these spaces24,25. People in urban areas with less green space have poorer health, but they are likely to benefit more by spending more time in nature or living in areas with more green space26.

In addition, the appearance of green spaces may change considerably with the seasons, thus affecting people's perceptions and preferences. Different weather conditions during the same season can also affect preferences27. However, few studies have considered the effects of seasonal dynamics on landscape perception28,29,30. Most studies have introduced participants to landscapes in warmer seasons, such as spring and summer31,32. Song et al. reported that walking around and viewing urban parks and urban neighbourhoods in spring, autumn, and winter had positive physical and psychological restorative effects33,34,35. Bielinis E et al. reported that during the winter months, short exposure to natural environments enhanced positive emotions among participants36.

There are two major limitations of research on landscape perception and experience. First, some studies have shown that despite the positive effects of exposure to natural images, such natural exposure may be detrimental to human well-being in the long run37,38. Second, almost all of the existing studies were conducted in the summer months or used photographs of natural landscapes in the summer. Therefore, it is unclear whether the health benefits associated with contact with natural environments can also be obtained in colder seasons39,40. The winter season is a typical climatic season, and a concomitant decrease in the number of people participating in physical activity has an impact on people's outdoor activities, leading to a gradual decline in the extent to which people experience natural landscapes41; however, the desire to restore physical and mental health does not diminish in the winter. The lack of information about people's perceptions and preferences during the winter months limits the recreational quality of urban green spaces and potential improvements40. Therefore, it is necessary to compare visual perception strategies between the summer and winter seasons.

This study incorporated site perception approaches for both the winter and summer seasons and used two experiential forms: seated viewing and walking tours. Psychophysiological indicators such as skin conductance (EDA), ear-tip pulse (PPG), and the Perception of the Environment Scale (PRS) were used to monitor participants' reactions to viewing four plant community types (lawn, forest, forest-shrub-grass, and forest-grass) and plazas with no plants (the control group); additionally, physical and psychological recovery were monitored. The results of this study could inform strategies for designing planted landscapes in urban parks and assessing the perceptual experience of landscapes with plant communities, thereby establishing a scientific foundation for future landscape perception efforts.

Our study aimed to investigate the following questions:

  1. (1)

    What are the effects of perceiving two seasonal landscapes on participants' physical and psychological recovery?

  2. (2)

    How do different seasonal plant community landscapes affect participants' perceptual experiences?

  3. (3)

    What impact does the type of plant community have on participants' physiological recovery and emotional responses?

Materials and methods

Research area and objects

This study was conducted in Xi'an city (34°16′N, 108°54′E), the capital of Shaanxi Province, which has a total area of 10,752 either square kilometers and a population of 12.6 million. The area of green coverage in this city is approximately 33.5%42. In this study, a park in Xi'an with a landscape of plant communities and different vegetation structures was selected as the study area. Green spaces with coloured foliage were excluded to ensure that variations in perception and preference were due to vegetation structure rather than other landscape attributes.

In this study, green spaces were created with combinations of three levels of vegetation: trees, shrubs, and herbs.. The vegetation types include single layers that create an open green space (e.g., grassland, single-layer woodland), semiopen green space formed by combinations of trees and grass (e.g., understorey and overstorey landscapes), and closed green spaces created by combinations of trees, shrubs, and grasses.

The morphologies and spatial attributes of the landscape spaces were used to distinguish the different vegetation structures; in total, four vegetation structures and a control group without plants were selected. The vegetation structures included single-layer grassland; single-layer woodland; tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands; and tree-grass composite woodland. Concrete squares without plants were present in the park, and the four vegetation structure types were chosen because they are common in Xi'an (Figs. 1 and 2, and Table 1).

Figure 1
figure 1

Study area. (Figure source: Base map from Autonavi Navigation; figure created by the first author. 2024 Autonavi Navigation—GS(2024)1158, and https://www.amap.com/search?query=%E6%96%87%E6%99%AF%E5%85%AC%E5%9B%AD&city=610100&geoobj=108.932943%7C34.320155%7C108.939129%7C34.322729&zoom=17.87).

Figure 2
figure 2

Research object. (Figure source: The image was created and photographed by the first author).

Table 1 Different vegetation types in green spaces.

Test object

A total of 400 college students (M age = 21.85, SD age = 3.34, age range = 17–25 years) were recruited as participants in this study. All the participants were healthy students and spoke Chinese. All participants were informed about the experimental procedures, associated risks, and confidentiality issues, and all signed an informed consent form before the experiment. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants were randomly assigned to one of 10 groups, with each group consisting of 40 individuals and corresponding to a specific combination of vegetation type and season in green spaces.

Physiopsychological indicators

Physiological measurement indicators

In this study, skin conductance (EDA) was used to monitor the skin conductance level (SCL) of the subjects and to record in real time the changes in physiological stress and comfort during the stress task and during the tour of different plant community types.

SCL has been shown to be positively correlated with sympathetic nerve activity in humans and is often used as a measure of stress; to a certain extent, SCL can reflect elevated levels of stress in individuals. Significant increases in SCL typically indicate a transition from a stable to a stressful state, while decreases suggest a move towards relaxation and calmness43.

Perceived restorativeness scale (PRS)

Kaplan summarised 4 attributes of restorative environments that can stimulate people's mental states and support recovery, namely, being away, coherence, fascination and compatibility44. Participants rated the characteristics of the four landscapes on a 5-point scale ranging from -2 to 2, with positive scores indicating better recovery from mental fatigue and negative scores suggesting increased mental fatigue. The PRS is currently considered a more general method for evaluating the degree of recovery from mental fatigue.

Experimental design

Test procedures

We began by briefly explaining the experiment to the participants and obtaining their written informed consent. After completing a short demographic questionnaire, participants were taken to the test scenario area, where electrodes were attached to their skin to continuously monitor and measure their SCL throughout the trial. Participants were then asked to relax for 3 min. The mean SCL during this relaxation period was used as the baseline SCL for assessing the impacts of the test scenarios and was recorded as M0. Participants subsequently completed a 3-min mathematical test to induce stress45. The mean SCL during the test was used to represent the SCL of the participants under stress, after which they completed an initial PRS assessment. This stage was recorded as M1 Each participant was then randomly assigned to one of five scenes, in which participants were placed in a seated viewing position and asked to wear handmade glasses, which were used to limit the range of the participant's visual field. Participants were shown the scenes in a seated position for 3 min36, and their baseline mean SCL was measured and recorded. This phase was recorded as M2. After viewing the scene in a seated position, the participant took a 1-min break and again performed a walking tour, which included a slow walk around the scene. Following the walking tours of the scene, participants completed the final PRS assessment, which was recorded as M3. After removing the SCL measurement equipment, participants left the test site. The duration of the experiments ranged from 0.4 to 0.5 h.

(a) Winter sensory experience.

The winter trial, conducted from 1 to 30 November 2020, had an average temperature of 11.2 °C (11.2 ± 1.27), with clear skies throughout the trial period, and the weather was clear (no rain). In order to reduce the impact of interfering variables, we ensured that the surroundings were quiet and that the light, temperature, humidity, and wind speed were consistent across the landscape areas. Before the test began, notices were posted within 2 m of the test site to inform visitors of the ongoing study, thereby minimising potential interference from external factors such as visitors' activities and noise.

(b) Summer sensory experience.

The summer trial, conducted from 1 to 30 June 2021, had a mean temperature of 26.8 °C (26.8 ± 2.75), and the weather remained clear throughout the trial period. The summer trial employed the same protocol for controlling external confounding variables as the winter trial.

Stressful tasks

To induce stress, participants were asked to complete a 3-min mathematical test. We told participants that the purpose of this experiment was to assess their performance on the numerical calculations, and we scored and ranked their performance to reflect participants’ physiological recovery and emotional changes more clearly after the experience. We used numerical calculations and simulations of noisy environments to induce psychological and physiological responses. Previous studies have demonstrated the effectiveness of noisy environments as stressors through SCL or emotional responses, showing that stressors can increase SCL or suppress mood, respectively46.

Data analysis

Data were processed using IMB SPSS Statistics 26.0 software (IBM, Inc., Armonk, NY, USA), and the changes in the relevant physiological and psychological indicators were calculated using the following formulas:

(1) Changes in physiological recovery at each stage.

Stress pressure value (M1) = M1-M0,

Sitting excursion value (M2) = M2-M0,

Walking tour value (M3) = M3-M0,

Recovery value (M4) = M3 + M2-M1.

(2) PRS change value.

PRS change value (∆M`1) = M` before experience—M` after experience.

We tested the effects of the two perception methods and the four vegetation structure types on physical and mental recovery by comparing the recovery values (ΔM4) of the different vegetation structures using one-way ANOVA for each perception method. We assessed the impact of each vegetation structure type on perceived environmental restorativeness by conducting a paired-samples t test to compare dimension scores before and after the experience.

Ethical statement

All subjects gave their informed consent for inclusion before they participated in the study. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the College of Architecture, Chang'an University.

Results

Effect of vegetation structure on participants' physiological recovery (SCL) across seasons

Effect of summer perception on participants' SCL

During the summer, our study of green spaces with varying vegetation structures revealed a decreasing trend in participants' SCL values after viewing single-layer grassland, single-layer woodland, and single-storey woodland in the seated viewing phase, suggesting a gradual reduction in stress compared to the stress phase (ΔM1). Participants exhibited a significant decrease in SCL values after viewing single-storey woodland and tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands. During the walking tour stage (ΔM3), SCL values in participants who viewed single-layer grassland and single-layer woodland gradually declined, with a more significant decrease observed among those who viewed the single-layer woodland compared to the seated viewing stage. For participants who viewed tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands and concrete squares without plants, an increasing trend in SCL values was observed. Conversely, participants who viewed tree-grass composite woodlands experienced an increasing trend in SCL values throughout the experiment, indicating a gradual increase in stress. During the seated viewing phase, participants exposed to concrete squares without plants initially showed a brief decrease in SCL values, but these values gradually increased during the walking tour phase, leading to an overall increase in stress. Our findings indicate that viewing and walking in the single-layer woodland during the summer were associated with higher levels of stress, whereas the body appeared to be significantly more relaxed in the seated position (Fig. 3a).

Figure 3
figure 3

Effect of perceptual experience on SCL in different seasons ((a) Summer perception, (b) Winter perception).

Effects of winter perception on participants' SCL

Our study of green spaces with various vegetation structures in winter revealed that, compared to the stress phase (∆M1), participants' SCL values generally decreased during the seated viewing phase (∆M2) after being exposed to single-layer grassland, single-layer woodland, tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands, and concrete squares without plants. A significant decrease in SCL values was observed after participants viewed single-layer grassland, single-layer woodland, and concrete squares without plants. In the walking tour phase (ΔM3), participants' SCL values started to gradually decrease compared to the seated viewing phase. For the remaining landscape types (single-layer grassland, single-layer woodland, and concrete squares without plants), participants' SCL values exhibited an increasing trend. Throughout the experiment, participants who viewed tree-grass composite woodlands experienced an increase in SCL values. Participants' SCL values showed varying responses to concrete squares without plants between winter and summer. Viewing concrete and asphalt squares without plants resulted in a shorter relaxation period for participants, and prolonged exposure to this environment led to an increased stress response. In contrast, walking among single-layer woodlands in winter was more effective for stress relief and resulted in a significant and noticeable relaxation of the body (Fig. 3b).

Effects of viewing green spaces with different vegetation structures on perceived environmental restorativeness among participants

Effect of summer perception on the perceived restorativeness of the environment

Paired t test results for the four dimensions of the environmental perception scale before and after exposure in the summer are presented in Fig. 4. Our analysis revealed that green spaces with different vegetation structures achieved higher postmeasurement scores on all four dimensions compared to concrete and asphalt squares without plants. Among the four vegetation structure types, single- layer woodland and tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands had higher scores for the four dimensions.

Figure 4
figure 4

The impact of environmental perception on PRS scores in summer ((a) Being away score, (b) extend score, (c) fascination score, (d) compatibility score).

(1) Being away.

After viewing tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands and tree-grass composite woodlands, participants' being away scores were significantly higher than before exposure to green spaces (p < 0.05). Participants' being away scores also increased after experiencing single-layer grassland and single-layer woodland compared to their scores before exposure to the green spaces, but the increasing trend was smaller than that for tree-shrub-grass composite and tree-grass composite woodlands. The being away scores decreased when participants were exposed to concrete squares without plants. After participants experienced the different scenarios, their M`1 values were ranked from largest to smallest as follows: tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands, tree-grass composite woodlands, single-layer woodland, single-layer grassland, concrete squares without plants (Fig. 4a).

(2) Extent.

The extent scores of all five scenario types showed an upward trend compared to the pre-experience scores, with significant differences observed for single-layer grassland, single-layer woodland, and tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands (p < 0.05). The concrete squares without plants had higher scores than the green spaces with different vegetation structures, while the latter had extent scores greater than those of the concrete squares without plants. After participants experienced the different scenarios, their M`1 values were ranked from largest to smallest as follows: single-layer woodland, tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands, single-layer grassland, tree-grass composite woodlands, concrete squares without plants (Fig. 4b).

(3) Fascination.

Fascination scores generally increased after participants viewed the five landscape types compared to their preexposure scores, with significant differences observed for single-layer woodland, tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands, and tree-grass composite woodlands (p < 0.05). Fascination scores were higher for the concrete squares without plants than for the green spaces with different vegetation structures. After participants experienced the different scenarios, their M`1 values were ranked from largest to smallest as follows: single-layer woodland, tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands, tree-grass composite woodlands, single-layer grassland, concrete squares without plants (Fig. 4c).

(4) Compatibility.

Compatibility scores generally increased after exposure to all four vegetation structures, whereas they tended to decrease for the concrete squares without plants. Compared to the pre-experience scores, the compatibility scores tended to increase after exposure to all four vegetation structures, while compatibility scores generally increased after exposure to all four vegetation structures, whereas they tended to decrease for the concrete squares without plants. Significant differences were observed between single-layer woodland and tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands before and after exposure (p < 0.05), with a more pronounced upwards trend in compatibility scores for these green spaces compared to single-layer grassland and tree-grass composite woodlands. After participants experienced the different scenarios, their M`1 values were ranked from largest to smallest as follows: single-layer woodland, tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands, single-layer grassland, tree-grass composite woodlands, concrete squares without plants (Fig. 4d).

Effects of winter on perceptions of environmental restorativeness

The winter trial exhibited less variation in pre- and postexperience scores compared to the summer trial, a pattern closely associated with the outdoor physical environment in winter. Paired t test results for the pre- and postexperience measurements of the four dimensions of the environmental perception scale in the winter trial are presented in Fig. 5. Our analysis revealed that for all four dimensions, participants' scores were higher after experiencing green spaces with different vegetation structures compared to concrete and asphalt squares without plants.

Figure 5
figure 5

Impact of winter environmental perception on PRSs ((a) Being away score, (b) Extent score, (c) Fascination score, (d) Compatibility score).

(a) Being away.

Compared to the pre-experience scores (∆M` pre-experience), participants' being away scores (∆M` postexperience) tended to increase after viewing single-layer woodlands, tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands, and tree-grassed composite woodlands. The being away scores tended to decrease after participants experienced single-layer grassland and concrete squares without plants, with the highest being away scores occurring after participants experienced single-layer woodland. The being away scores decreased significantly after participants experienced concrete squares without plants. After participants experienced the different scenarios, their M`1 values were ranked from largest to smallest as follows: single-layer woodland, tree-grass composite woodland, tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands, single-layer grassland, concrete squares without plants (Fig. 5a).

(b) Extent.

The participants' extent scores before and after experiencing the different scenarios were examined. The results revealed that significantly higher extent scores were observed after participants experienced the four vegetation structures compared to the concrete squares without plants. After experiencing the tree-grass composite woodland, participants rated the overall feeling of the scenario as better, followed by the tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands and single-layer woodland, while they reported the worst overall feeling after viewing the single-layer grassland. After participants experienced the different scenarios, their M`1 values were ranked from largest to smallest as follows: tree-grass composite woodlands, tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands, single-layer woodland, single-layer grassland, concrete squares without plants (Fig. 5b).

(c) Fascination.

Participants' fascination scores were significantly higher after experiencing the green spaces with different vegetation structures than after experiencing the concrete squares without plants, as indicated by our analysis of the pre- and postexperience ratings. Compared with the pre-experience rating (ΔM` before the experience), participants rated the scenarios as more favourable after viewing the single-layer woodland, tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands, and tree-grass composite woodlands; participants’ fascination scores decreased significantly after experiencing the single-layer grassland and concrete squares without plants. such as "Significant differences were found in the fascination scores before and after experiencing single-layer woodlands (p < 0.001), single-layer grassland (p < 0.01), and concrete squares without plants (p < 0.001). After participants experienced the different scenarios, their M`1 values were ranked from largest to smallest as follows: single-layer woodland, tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands, tree-grass composite woodlands, single-layer grassland, concrete squares without plants (Fig. 5c).

(d) Compatibility.

Analyses of participants' compatibility scores after experiencing the different experimental scenarios showed that, compared to the scores after experiencing concrete squares without plants, participants' compatibility scores after experiencing the single-layer woodlands and tree-grass composite woodlands were significantly greater, while the scores decreased after experiencing tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands and single-layer grassland. The compatibility scores showed an increasing trend after participants experienced single-storey woodland and tree-grass composite woodland, and a decreasing trend was observed after participants experienced single-storey grassland. Compatibility scores decreased significantly after participants experienced concrete squares without plants. In comparison to the pre-experience scores (M` pre-experience), the compatibility scores of the participants after experiencing the single-layer woodland (M` postexperience) were greater. After participants experienced the different scenarios, their M`1 values were ranked from largest to smallest as follows: single-layer woodland, tree-grass composite woodlands, tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands, single-layer grassland, concrete squares without plants (Fig. 5d).

Seasonal variation and the effect of green spaces with different vegetation structures on physiological recovery and perceived environmental resilience

Seasonal variation

Our one-way ANOVA results revealed a significant difference in perceived recovery values between the environment in summer and winter, with significant differences observed only in the Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS) for extent and compatibility (Table 2). Environmental perception scores for extent and compatibility were higher in summer compared to winter. Our findings suggest that the natural environment in summer is more conducive to restorative experiences for participants compared to winter.

Table 2 Changes in participants' physiological recovery and environmental perception scores after viewing green spaces with vegetation structures (excluding concrete squares without plants) in different seasons.

Effects of green spaces with different vegetation structures on physiological recovery and perceived environmental restorativeness

Our analyses revealed that vegetation type had a significant impact on recovery and emotional responses (being away and fascination) in green spaces, but not on Skin Conductance Level (SCL), as shown in Table 3.

Table 3 Effects of physiological recovery and emotional responses experienced in green spaces with different types of vegetation structures (containing concrete squares without plants).

Significant differences in being away scores were observed in only five cases, specifically between single-layer grassland and single-layer woodland (p = 0.002), single-layer woodland and tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands (p = 0.01), single-layer grassland and tree-grass composite woodlands (p = 0.042), single-layer woodland and concrete squares without plants (p = 0), tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands and concrete squares without plants (p = 0), and tree-grass composite woodlands and concrete and asphalt squares without plants (p = 0). Among the green spaces evaluated, single-layer woodlands exhibited the highest recovery value, followed by tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands, tree-grass composite woodlands, single-layer grassland, and concrete squares without plants.

Only six comparisons showed significant differences in scores: between single-layer grassland and single-layer woodland (p = 0), single-layer grassland and tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands (p = 0.006), single-layer woodland and tree-grass composite woodlands (p = 0.027), tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands and concrete squares without plants (p = 0), single-layer woodland and concrete squares without plants (p = 0), and tree-grass composite woodland and concrete squares without plants (p = 0.003). Among the green spaces assessed, single-layer woodlands demonstrated the highest recovery value, followed by tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands, tree-grass composite woodlands, single-layer grassland, and concrete squares without plants.

Effects of seasonal changes in green spaces with different vegetation structures on participants' physiological recovery and perceptions of environmental resilience

(1) Effects of seasonal changes in green spaces with different vegetation structures on participants' physiological recovery.

Our paired t tests revealed a significant increase in participants' physiological recovery values (ΔM4) in summer when viewing single-layer woodlands, tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands, and tree-grass composite woodlands, compared to their physiological recovery values in winter. Notably, participants' physiological recovery values (ΔM4) showed a significant increase after viewing the tree-grass composite woodland in summer. Regardless of season, viewing tree-grass composite woodlands consistently had a positive impact on participants' physiological recovery, as illustrated in Fig. 6.

Figure 6
figure 6

Seasonal changes in green spaces with different vegetation structures on participants' SCLs.

(2) Effect of seasonal changes in green spaces with different vegetation structures on the perception of environmental restorativeness.

PRSs for green spaces with different vegetation structures were generally lower in winter than in summer, reflecting the influence of the outdoor physical environment on these perceptions. Figure 7 shows the results of our investigation into the four dimensions of the PRSs for green spaces with different vegetation structures in both summer and winter. Our results indicate that green spaces with different vegetation structures were perceived differently across the four dimensions of the PRSs in winter and summer, and in both seasons, PRSs for green spaces with vegetation were higher than for concrete squares without plants.

Figure 7
figure 7

The effect of seasonal changes in green spaces with different vegetation structures on participants' PRSs.

(a) Being away.

The being away scores were significantly higher for concrete squares without plants in the summer compared to the winter (p = 0.012), but there were no significant seasonal differences in being away scores for green spaces with different vegetation structures. However, the being away scores were greater for the single-layer woodland in the winter than for the single-layer woodland in the summer (Fig. 7a).

(b) Extent.

After experiencing single-layer grassland and single-layer woodland in the summer, participants had significantly higher extent scores than when they experienced these landscapes in the winter. Specifically, the single-layer woodland received the highest ratings, followed closely by the single-layer grassland, with p-values of 0.005 and 0.02, respectively (Fig. 7b).

(c) Fascination.

We analyzed the fascination scores of participants who experienced green spaces with different vegetation structures in various seasons. Our results revealed that participants had significantly higher fascination scores in the summer for these green spaces than in the winter. In particular, participants enjoyed the scenes more after viewing the single-layer grassland (p = 0.01) or concrete squares without plants (p = 0.002) in the summer. Moreover, the fascination scores for single-layer woodland in the winter were higher than those in the summer (Fig. 7c).

(d) Compatibility.

Analysis of compatibility scores revealed that participants who experienced tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands in the summer had significantly higher compatibility scores than those who experienced green spaces with different vegetation structures in the winter. In contrast, participants who experienced tree-grass composite woodlands in the winter had slightly higher compatibility scores than those who experienced green spaces with different vegetation structures in the summer (Fig. 7d).

Discussion

Effects of different seasons on physiological recovery and environmental perceptual resilience among participants

Environmental perception varied significantly between seasons, indicating that exposure to natural environments in summer was more beneficial for participants. Significant differences in participants' physiological recovery between seasons were mainly attributed to the restorative aspects of environmental perception (being away, extent, compatibility), and no significant differences were observed in physiological recovery. Exposure to natural environments during winter led to a decrease in participants' SCL values, whereas in summer, such exposure resulted in an increase in their extent and fascination scores, indicating that summer landscapes were more effective at relaxing and soothing participants' emotions compared to winter landscapes, which had a less pronounced effect on reducing stress levels. Landscape perceptions can be significantly affected by seasonal variables, potentially because changes in plant biology during seasonal transitions alter the visual and ecological characteristics of vegetation, such as color, shape, density, and biodiversity, which in turn influence visual perceptions and psychological responses27,47. The established relationship between colour-induced visual stimulation and psychological functioning suggests that leaf colour variations across different landscape types, particularly forests, have a significant impact on landscape aesthetics and human perceptual and physiological states48,49,50,51. Therefore, plant species within the vegetation cycle (summer) and outside the vegetation cycle (winter) were considered, and the leaf colour of the selected plant species was considered green.

Seasonal changes primarily affect the human body through alterations in the physical environment, including temperature, humidity, and light. Significant differences in human comfort arise from temperature variations between winter and summer, with the main impact being on the body's ability to recover from mental fatigue, as evidenced by the scores on the environmental perception dimensions. When evaluating their ability to recover from mental fatigue, participants will independently assess green spaces with varying vegetation structures, taking into account not only sensory factors but also the influence of the surrounding environment, temperature, and humidity. People experience a greater sense of relaxation in summer landscapes, leading to enhanced abilities to recover from mental fatigue compared to winter. Conversely, the bleakness of winter landscapes tends to diminish people's ability to recover from mental fatigue52. As a result, the perceptual experience of summer landscapes is more profound than that of winter landscapes.

Seasonal changes in green spaces with different vegetation structures affect participants' landscape experiences

Concrete squares without plants elicited lower PRSs, likely due to the hard material's reminder of indoor environments where participants spend much of their time working and living. In summer, the PRSs for concrete squares without plants were higher than in winter, a phenomenon attributed to the vibrant colours and diverse vegetation structures present in the surroundings53. While the PRSs for concrete squares without plants were higher in summer than in winter, the overall perception of environmental restoration was not significantly favoured by participants. Further research is required to ascertain if enhancing the planted landscape around concrete squares without plants in future landscape designs could significantly boost the restorative effect of environmental perception. Seasonal variations in PRSs among participants may be influenced by the monotony of single-layer grassland, with higher scores reported in summer and lower scores in winter. Our findings indicate that the restorative power of single-layer grassland is primarily driven by visual comfort, encompassing the impacts of the landscape and sunlight on individuals' perceptions54. Research has demonstrated that seasonal changes can impact people's connection with nature in urban settings55, and some studies have posited that winter may negatively affect people's well-being indices56. Walking in urban parks during winter has been found to boost positive emotions33; yet people's perception of environmental restorativeness remains largely based on subjective feelings, and single-layer grasslands do not adequately shield against the cold in winter.

Tree-grass composite woodlands showed comparable PRSs in winter and summer, aligning with previous research on seasonal preferences57. This may be due to the attractiveness of the tree-grass composite woodlands. The tree-grass composite woodlands have a positive aesthetic quality, with a calm and relaxing atmosphere but a low level of approachability, which prevents participants from entering the space to experience and feel the landscape. Landscape preference is influenced by several perceptual scales, including spaciousness, naturalisation, species richness, and shelter58. In winter, single-layer woodlands scored higher on 'being away' and 'fascination' compared to summer, while in summer, they received higher ratings for 'extent'. The differences were statistically significant (p = 0.01). The visual characteristics of plants in different seasons affect landscape perception59; these characteristics include physical factors, such as the angle of incoming radiation, which is much less direct in winter than in summer and creates dappled shadows in single-layer woodlands, which enriches landscape interest and aesthetics. Tree shading is known to decrease surface temperatures and enhance latent heat exchange through evapotranspiration60. Beyond visual appreciation, quietness stands out as the most cherished attribute of green spaces, closely followed by spaciousness61, and single-layer woodland environments in winter are relatively quiet, allowing for a sense of relaxation. Additionally, the composition of plant species in single-layer woodlands is predominantly pine and cypress, and these trees release volatile organic compounds that have a calming effect on human emotions and physiology49. This observation underscores the seasonal differences in 'being away', 'fascination', and 'extent' within this type of landscape. PRSs for tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands were consistently higher during summer compared to winter, indicating that a balance of openness, moderate vegetation density, and biodiversity is essential to enhance the appeal of these landscapes62, fulfilling the participants' desires for scenic views and a sense of security63. Compared to single-layer woodlands, tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands exhibit a greater density. While this can sometimes hinder visibility and perceived safety, it also enhances privacy and provides ample shade, leading to higher PRSs in summer.

Conclusions

In this study, we monitored participants' physiological recovery and psychological well-being based on their perception and experience of green spaces with different seasonal vegetation structures. This study aims to establish a scientific foundation for understanding how landscape perception and experience impact individuals and to serve as a theoretical guide for future landscape planning. Significant differences in perception were observed between the two seasons, with participants exposed to the summer landscape showing reduced SCL values and those in the winter landscape exhibiting higher scores for extent and compatibility. Second, exposure to single-layer woodlands and tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands in summer, as well as single-layer woodlands in winter, resulted in restorative effects for participants. Next, among the vegetation types studied, single-layer woodlands provided the most positive perceptual experience. In terms of perceptual experience, tree-shrub-grass composite woodlands came second, followed by tree-grass composite woodlands, single-layer grassland, and finally, concrete squares without vegetation. Lastly, the findings emphasize that these factors are distinct and not interchangeable. It is advisable for future landscape design to take into account the seasonal variations in vegetation when selecting plant species. Evergreen single-layer woodlands would be an ideal choice for winter urban landscapes.

This study has several limitations. First, all the participants were university students, so the results may not reflect other social groups. These results should be validated in other groups. In addition, the age range of the participants was between 17 and 25 years, and the results may differ for individuals of other ages. The age of the participants can be used as a variable in future research. Second, while the four vegetation types included in this study are common and representative of green spaces, they do not cover the full spectrum of possible vegetation structures. For a more comprehensive understanding of how different vegetation structures affect individuals, it is necessary to investigate additional vegetation types or plant species. Lastly, since the research was limited to winter and summer, the participants' preferences might have been biased by seasonal influences. Future research could extend this work to include all four seasons for a more complete comparison.