Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

Imprinting sets the stage for speciation

Abstract

Sexual imprinting—a phenomenon in which offspring learn parental traits and later use them as a model for their own mate preferences—can generate reproductive barriers between species1. When the target of imprinting is a mating trait that differs among young lineages, imprinted preferences may contribute to behavioural isolation and facilitate speciation1,2. However, in most models of speciation by sexual selection, divergent natural selection is also required; the latter acts to generate and maintain variation in the sexually selected trait or traits, and in the mating preferences that act upon them3. Here we demonstrate that imprinting, in addition to mediating female mate preferences, can shape biases in male–male aggression. These biases can act similarly to natural selection to maintain variation in traits and mate preferences, which facilitates reproductive isolation driven entirely by sexual selection. Using a cross-fostering study, we show that both male and female strawberry poison frogs (Oophaga pumilio) imprint on coloration, which is a mating trait that has diverged recently and rapidly in this species4. Cross-fostered females prefer to court mates of the same colour as their foster mother, and cross-fostered males are more aggressive towards rivals that share the colour of their foster mother. We also use a simple population-genetics model to demonstrate that when both male aggression biases and female mate preferences are formed through parental imprinting, sexual selection alone can (1) stabilize a sympatric polymorphism and (2) strengthen the trait–preference association that leads to behavioural reproductive isolation. Our study provides evidence of imprinting in an amphibian and suggests that this rarely considered combination of rival and sexual imprinting can reduce gene flow between individuals that bear divergent mating traits, which sets the stage for speciation by sexual selection.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Map of the Bocas del Toro archipelago, showing the diversity of colour and pattern among populations of O. pumilio.
Fig. 2: O. pumilio rearing experiment.
Fig. 3: Polymorphism stability and associations between trait and preference.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study have been deposited in Figshare (https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.9628406).

Code availability

Code files for statistical analysis (R) and mathematical models (Mathematica) have been deposited in Figshare (https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.9628406).

References

  1. Irwin, D. E. & Price, T. Sexual imprinting, learning and speciation. Heredity 82, 347–354 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Ritchie, M. G. Sexual selection and speciation. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 38, 79–102 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Servedio, M. R. & Boughman, J. W. The role of sexual selection in local adaptation and speciation. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 48, 85–109 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Brown, J. L., Maan, M. E., Cummings, M. E. & Summers, K. Evidence for selection on coloration in a Panamanian poison frog: a coalescent-based approach. J. Biogeogr. 37, 891–901 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Felsenstein, J. Skepticism towards Santa Rosalia, or why are there so few kinds of animals? Evolution 35, 124–138 (1981).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. van Doorn, G. S., Dieckmann, U. & Weissing, F. J. Sympatric speciation by sexual selection: a critical reevaluation. Am. Nat. 163, 709–725 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Arnegard, M. E. & Kondrashov, A. S. Sympatric speciation by sexual selection alone is unlikely. Evolution 58, 222–237 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Verzijden, M. N. et al. The impact of learning on sexual selection and speciation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 27, 511–519 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Servedio, M. R., Van Doorn, G. S., Kopp, M., Frame, A. M. & Nosil, P. Magic traits in speciation: ‘magic’ but not rare? Trends Ecol. Evol. 26, 389–397 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Boughman, J. W. How sensory drive can promote speciation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 17, 571–577 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Seehausen, O. & Schluter, D. Male–male competition and nuptial-colour displacement as a diversifying force in Lake Victoria cichlid fishes. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 271, 1345–1353 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Dijkstra, P. D. & Border, S. E. How does male–male competition generate negative frequency-dependent selection and disruptive selection during speciation? Curr. Zool. 64, 89–99 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Berglund, A., Bisazza, A. & Pilastro, A. Armaments and ornaments: an evolutionary explanation of traits of dual utility. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 58, 385–399 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Grether, G. F., Peiman, K. S., Tobias, J. A. & Robinson, B. W. Causes and consequences of behavioral interference between species. Trends Ecol. Evol. 32, 760–772 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Mikami, O. K., Kohda, M. & Kawata, M. A new hypothesis for species coexistence: male–male repulsion promotes coexistence of competing species. Popul. Ecol. 46, 213–217 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Hansen, B. T. & Slagsvold, T. Rival imprinting: interspecifically cross-fostered tits defend their territories against heterospecific intruders. Anim. Behav. 65, 1117–1123 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Verzijden, M. N., Korthof, R. E. M. & Ten Cate, C. Females learn from mothers and males learn from others. The effect of mother and siblings on the development of female mate preferences and male aggression biases in Lake Victoria cichlids, genus Mbipia. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62, 1359–1368 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Summers, K., Cronin, T. W. & Kennedy, T. Variation in spectral reflectance among populations of Dendrobates pumilio, the strawberry poison frog, in the Bocas del Toro Archipelago, Panama. J. Biogeogr. 30, 35–53 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Dugas, M. B. & Richards-Zawacki, C. L. A captive breeding experiment reveals no evidence of reproductive isolation among lineages of a polytypic poison frog. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 116, 52–62 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Richards-Zawacki, C. L., Wang, I. J. & Summers, K. Mate choice and the genetic basis for colour variation in a polymorphic dart frog: inferences from a wild pedigree. Mol. Ecol. 21, 3879–3892 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Yang, Y., Richards-Zawacki, C. L., Devar, A. & Dugas, M. B. Poison frog color morphs express assortative mate preferences in allopatry but not sympatry. Evolution 70, 2778–2788 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Saporito, R. A., Zuercher, R., Roberts, M., Kenneth, G. & Donnelly, M. A. Experimental evidence for aposematism in the dendrobatid poison frog Oophaga pumilio. Copeia 2007, 1006–1011 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Hegna, R. H., Saporito, R. A. & Donnelly, M. A. Not all colors are equal: predation and color polytypism in the aposematic poison frog Oophaga pumilio. Evol. Ecol. 27, 831–845 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Richards-Zawacki, C. L., Yeager, J. & Bart, H. P. S. No evidence for differential survival or predation between sympatric color morphs of an aposematic poison frog. Evol. Ecol. 27, 783–795 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Rojas, B. Behavioural, ecological, and evolutionary aspects of diversity in frog colour patterns. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 92, 1059–1080 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Maan, M. E. & Cummings, M. E. Female preferences for aposematic signal components in a polymorphic poison frog. Evolution 62, 2334–2345 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Yang, Y., Dugas, M. B., Sudekum, H. J., Murphy, S. & Richards-Zawacki, C. L. Male–male aggression is unlikely to stabilize a poison frog polymorphism. J. Evol. Biol. 31, 457–468 (2018).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Dugas, M. B. Simple observations with complex implications: what we have learned and can learn about parental care from a frog that feeds its young. Zool. Anz. 273, 192–202 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Uy, J. A. C., Moyle, R. G., Filardi, C. E. & Cheviron, Z. A. Difference in plumage color used in species recognition between incipient species is linked to a single amino acid substitution in the melanocortin-1 receptor. Am. Nat. 174, 244–254 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Hoekstra, H. E., Hirschmann, R. J., Bundey, R. A., Insel, P. A. & Crossland, J. P. A single amino acid mutation contributes to adaptive beach mouse color pattern. Science 313, 101–104 (2006).

    Article  CAS  ADS  Google Scholar 

  31. Verzijden, M. N., Lachlan, R. F. & Servedio, M. R. Female mate-choice behavior and sympatric speciation. Evolution 59, 2097–2108 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Gosner, K. L. A simplified table for staging anuran embryos larvae with notes on identification. Herpetologica 16, 183–190 (1960).

    Google Scholar 

  33. Kirkpatrick, P. Sexual selection and the evolution of female mate choice. Evolution 36, 1–12 (1982).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Wolfram Research. Mathematica 11.1. https://reference.wolfram.com/legacy/language/v11.1/ (2018).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank A. Benge, A. Devar, D. Gonzalez, R. Cossio, M. Dugas, T. Stetzinger, J. Yeager and G. Zawacki for help with the rearing experiments, and V. Prémel, J. P. Lawrence, S. A. Echeverri and I. J. Wang for providing photographs for the O. pumilio colour morphs. The Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute (STRI) provided logistical support for this project, and we are particularly grateful to G. Jacome and P. Gondola of the Bocas del Toro Field Station. This research was supported by the Smithsonian Institution, the University of California President’s Office, Tulane University’s Newcomb College Institute and the National Science Foundation (award numbers OISE-0701165 and DEB-1146370 to C.L.R.-Z. and DEB-1255777 to M.R.S.). The Panamanian National Authority for the Environment (ANAM) provided research and export permission for this study. This work complied with IACUC protocols (STRI no. 2007-17-12-15-07 and Tulane no. 0382).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

C.L.R.-Z. designed the rearing experiment and collected data, and Y.Y. carried out statistical analyses, for the breeding experiment. Y.Y. and M.R.S. performed the theoretical research. Y.Y. drafted the manuscript, and M.R.S. and C.L.R.-Z. contributed to writing and revision.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yusan Yang.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Peer review information Nature thanks Peter Dijkstra, Leithen M’Gonigle and Machteld Nicolette Verzijden for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Extended data figures and tables

Extended Data Fig. 1 Effects of starting frequency.

Simulations of 2,000 generations demonstrate the effects of the starting frequencies of alleles on the maintenance of polymorphism. Each panel represents a combination of a particular starting frequency for T1 (t1, labelled on top), association between mating trait and behaviour (phenogenotypic linkage disequilibrium (LD) between the trait genotype and the behavioural phenotype, labelled on the right) (Supplementary Information), and types of imprinting (maternal or paternal, labelled on the right). Within each panel, we ran every combination of α and β from 0.01 to 100, with a step size of 100.1. Axes are on logarithmic scales.

Extended Data Fig. 2 Frequency of the trait-1 phenotype at stable polymorphic equilibrium.

In this extended model, the value of α and β varies with the phenotype on which the individual imprinted (Supplementary Information). Frequency of the trait-1 phenotype includes T11 and T12 individuals (x1x2x4x5). Each panel represents a particular combination of α1 and β1 from the set {0.01, 1, 100}, labelled on the top and the right. Within each panel, we ran every combination of α2 and β2 from 0.01 to 100, with a step size of 100.2. Axes are on logarithmic scales. The white area in each panel is the parameter space in which no stable polymorphism can be found. The frequency of the trait-1 phenotype at polymorphic equilibrium for a given combination of αk and βk is slightly different between the models of maternal and paternal imprinting (<0.1, not shown).

Extended Data Fig. 3 Dcor at stable polymorphic equilibrium.

In this extended model, the value of α and β varies with the phenotype that the individual imprinted on (Supplementary Information). Trait–behaviour linkage disequilibrium between the trait genotype and the behavioural phenotype (Dcor, calculated as D/√(p1p2t1t2)) at the stable polymorphic equilibrium. Each panel represents a particular combination of α1 and β1 from the set {0.01, 1, 100}, labelled on the top and the right. Within each panel, we ran combinations of α2 and β2 from 0.01 to 100, with a step size of 100.2. Axes are on logarithmic scales. The figure presents results from maternal imprinting models. Overall, the paternal imprinting models produced higher Dcor at polymorphic equilibrium than did the maternal imprinting models, but the differences were very small (<0.1, not shown).

Extended Data Table 1 Proportions of association time for the three groups of rearing treatment
Extended Data Table 2 Proportions of approaches for the three groups of rearing treatment

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

This file contains Supplementary section S1 (rearing experiment), Supplementary section S2 (population genetic models) and associated references.

Reporting Summary

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Yang, Y., Servedio, M.R. & Richards-Zawacki, C.L. Imprinting sets the stage for speciation. Nature 574, 99–102 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-019-1599-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-019-1599-z

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing