Introduction

Apathy: a complex clinical, neurobehavioral and neurobiological construct

Most clinicians caring for older adults recognize that apathy is common in late life, particularly among individuals with depression and dementia. Presence of apathy affects psychiatric and neurological outcomes [1, 2], takes a toll on patients and their families [3], and may herald cognitive and functional decline [4]. Yet many questions about apathy remain regarding its phenomenology, clinical characterization, neuropsychological and neurobiological underpinnings. Is apathy a neuropsychiatric symptom or a behavioral response? Is apathy the same construct in depression as it is in dementia? How does one assess it? How does one conceptualize apathy from a clinical neuroscience perspective? Much of what we know about neural correlates of apathy relates to post-stroke conditions [5], so to what extent does vascular neuropathology play a role in the evolution of apathy?

In this paper, we provide an updated review of the literature on apathy, with a particular focus on its role in depression and neurocognitive impairment, both major (dementia) and minor (syndromes related to mild cognitive impairment). While our goal is to provide a clinical neuroscience-based overview of neurobiological and neuropsychological phenomena related to apathy, we feel it is important to start with a clinical perspective. Given the outstanding questions about presentation of apathy, its characterization and assessment, its impact on a variety of medical and psychiatric conditions, and its treatment, beginning with a review of clinical issues surrounding apathy should provide a strong foundation from which to build a clinical neuroscience understanding of its neurobiological and neurobehavioral basis.

Apathy: a clinical perspective

Apathy is a common and pervasive neuropsychiatric syndrome [6]. It was originally conceptualized as a “lack of motivation” and has since gained more dimensions as academic endeavors and expert panels have sought to define it [6, 7]. In 2018, apathy was defined as a quantitative reduction of goal-directed activity when compared with a patient’s previous level of functioning. The presentation must persist for at least four weeks and affect at least two of the three apathy dimensions: behavior/cognition; emotion; social interaction [6]. More recently, in 2021, these domains were updated to diminished initiative, diminished interest and diminished emotional expression/responsiveness [7]. Specific diagnostic criteria are shown in Tables 1 and 2.

Table 1 Diagnostic Criteria for Apathy and Diagnostic criteria for apathy in neurocognitive disorders [7] (reprinted with permission).
Table 2 2018 Consensus panel diagnostic criteria for apathy [6] (reprinted with permission).

Epidemiology

Prior studies have shown that the prevalence of apathy in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) ranges between 10.7% and 44.8% [8]. In a meta-analysis by Zhao et al, the most frequent neuropsychiatric symptom of Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) was shown to be apathy, with an overall prevalence of 49% (95% CI 41–57%) [9]. Apathy is also the most persistent neuropsychiatric symptom in AD patients [10]. In the behavioral variant of Frontotemporal Dementia (FTD), the prevalence of apathy is staggering, with ranges of 62 to 89% [11]. The prevalence of apathy in Dementia with Lewy Bodies ranges from 35 to 100% [12]. As a potential harbinger of dementia, apathy was linked to an approximately 2-fold increased risk of incident dementia in patients presenting to memory clinics [13]. Once dementia starts, apathy is associated with worsening functional decline, higher caregiver burden, early institutionalization with increased cost of care resulting, and increased mortality [14].

Clinical assessment

Whether considered a symptom or a standalone disorder, apathy has been the subject of much debate. Across the literature, many terms have been used to describe similar presentations to apathy: amotivation, avolition, abulia, withdrawal, social isolation, etc. To delineate a symptomatic apathy syndrome, consensus statements have been issued by expert groups and are detailed in Tables 1 and 2 (6, 7). Both sets of criteria call for the exclusion of other psychiatric disorders. This is especially difficult with depression, as it may have a similar presentation to apathy, especially in older adults. Both apathy and depression present with decreased interest, decreased initiative, decreased motivation, impaired concentration and libido, and reported fatigue. They are, however, distinct but often co-occurring syndromes [15, 16]. Brodaty and Connors suggested the following features to differentiate apathy and depression: In apathy, the lack of emotion and indifference, passivity, lack of rumination and anxiety, and increasing severity over the course of dementia set the presentation apart from depression wherein the patient would present dysphoric, hopeless, avoidant of treatment and socialization, comorbid with changes in sleep, appetite, possibly suicidal, and with a course that would fluctuate with medication trials and treatment [17]. In depression, an individual may still find social relationships to be valuable despite an absent drive to pursue them while a person with apathy no longer finds worth in social relationships [18].

To reliably assess apathy and track therapeutic interventions, use of validated scales is generally recommended, especially the Apathy Evaluation Scale and the Dementia Apathy Interview and Rating. The Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES) [19] measures apathy regardless of etiology or diagnosis and has been validated for use in a variety of clinical populations, including nursing home residents. It is sensitive to change over four weeks and capable of tracking apathy in three domains (cognitive, behavioral, and emotional). Importantly, its semi-structured interview format exists for all three information sources: patients themselves, caregivers and clinicians [19]. The Dementia Apathy Interview and Rating (DAIR) gathers information in a semi-structured interview conducted by the clinician with a patient or an informant, and is capable of tracking changes at four weeks, which is especially useful for monitoring outcomes of interventions for patients with Alzheimer’s Disease [20].

Other promising avenues look to technology to gather data that can supplement and inform clinical information such as passive environmental sensing technology.

Treatment

There are no established clinical guidelines for treatment of apathy [15]. With regards to nonpharmacological interventions, a systematic review found that therapeutic interventions such as stimulation, creative activities, Montessori methods, cooking, and individually tailored behavioral interventions yield positive outcomes. There was not a high level of evidence for music therapy, exercise groups, multisensory stimulation, or pet therapy [21]. “Engage,” a behavioral therapy aimed at reward exposure, was found to be effective for depression [22].

There are no FDA-approved medications for apathy. The utility of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) for apathy in depression is unclear. The literature on the topic includes several case reports that described onset or worsening of apathy with SSRIs in depression [16]. In one study, methylphenidate was not effective in enhancing SSRI treatment of apathy in depression [23]. Another study found that while bupropion was safe, it was not efficacious compared with placebo in the treatment of apathy in patients with Alzheimer’s dementia in the absence of clinically relevant depressed mood [24]. In Lewy Body Dementia, acetylcholinesterase inhibitors (AchEIs) were found to be useful in a systematic review of four studies [12]. For FTD, Hermann et al. found citalopram to be associated with a decrease in apathy on the Frontal Behavioral Inventory [25]. In a preliminary study, agomelatine, an antidepressant with MT1 and MT2 receptor agonism and 5-HT2C receptor antagonism, decreased apathy scores on the AES – clinician version (AES-C) in patients with FTD as compared to melatonin. The medication was well-tolerated and was also correlated with decrease in caregiver distress [26]. Looking to the future, a phase II trial is currently underway for the use of intranasal oxytocin for behavior in FTD [27].

For treatment studies of apathy in AD, we consider the ADMET trial. In this six-week, randomized, phase II, double-blind, placebo-controlled multicenter trial enrolling Alzheimer’s disease participants with apathy assigned to methylphenidate 20 mg daily or placebo, there was a statistically significant reduction in CGI-C and Neuropsychiatric Inventory apathy scores with active treatment, but there was no treatment difference on the AES [28]. A Phase III clinical trial, ADMET II, is currently underway [29]. In another study, AES-C scores were significantly improved (after adjusting for baseline apathy) with a trial of methylphenidate up to 20 mg daily in a 12-week, double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial in a cohort of older community-dwelling veterans [30]. A 2018 meta-analysis demonstrated further support for methylphenidate as a treatment for apathy [31]. Though some studies show slight improvement of apathy with cholinesterase inhibitors, the aforementioned meta-analysis did not find high-quality evidence to support the use of any cholinesterase inhibitor (donepezil, galantamine or rivastigmine) for apathy in Alzheimer’s [31]. There is no evidence to support the discontinuation of cholinesterase inhibitors as an intervention to decrease apathy either [24, 30]. Low-quality studies showed slight improvement of apathy with antipsychotic and antidepressant use in Alzheimer’s disease [31].

Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) was shown to improve AES-C scores significantly compared with sham treatment in a randomized, double-blind, parallel-arm, sham-controlled pilot study in subjects presenting with apathy in AD. The authors proposed several mechanisms for this finding, including an enhanced dopamine transmission, increased neuronal activity in prefrontal regions, and neurotrophic/neuroprotective effects when the DLPFC (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) is stimulated. This effect was significant at four weeks but not maintained at 8- or 12-week follow-up, possibly due to a small sample of 20 participants and short duration of treatment [32].

Apathy: a perspective linking neuropsychology and brain circuitry

The core definition of the apathy syndrome is a reduction in motivated goal-directed behavior. Engaging in motivated behavior requires a cost-benefit evaluation of whether the reward associated with that potential behavior outweighs the cost of performance [33]. Motivation to obtain rewards is clearly decreased in major depression [34]. Depressed patients are less willing to exert effort to obtain rewards and apathy appears to further reduce the incentive of rewarding outcomes [35, 36]. Motivated behavior also requires the engagement of executive functions – or the cognitive control mechanisms of response inhibition, working memory, and mental flexibility that sustain attention to the task at hand, engage monitoring and evaluation of task progress, and enable the ability to subsequently change direction if need be [37]. The behavioral syndrome of apathy is therefore thought to originate in brain regions responsible for both executive functioning and reward/motivation. For example, three recent reviews provide insight into imaging findings related to apathy in AD, pointing to frontostriatal circuit involvement, including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), prefrontal cortex (PFC) and parts of the basal ganglia, particularly the ventral striatum, including the nucleus accumbens and olfactory tubercle [38,39,40].

Three frontostriatal circuits underlie executive functioning and motivational states. These circuits include the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) circuit, the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) circuit. All three circuits originate in the prefrontal cortex and have direct and indirect pathways that project to the striatum, then to the globus pallidus and substantia nigra, and onto to the thalamus before looping back to the prefrontal cortex [41]. The three circuits run in close proximity to one another in parallel through shared common brain structures and receive inputs from neurotransmitter systems that help modulate behavior. Dopaminergic systems – in particular the mesolimbic and mesocortical pathways – assist in the regulation of motivational behavior and executive functioning [42]. The mesolimbic pathway begins in the ventral tegmental area and projects to the nucleus accumbens, limbic system, and the medial prefrontal cortex, while the mesocortical pathway connects the ventral tegmental area to the prefrontal cortex [43]. Overall, then, frontostriatal circuitry works in conjunction with dopaminergic pathways to allow for the effective pursuit of motivated behaviors [44].

Circuitry underlying executive functioning and apathy

The DLPFC circuit is closely associated with the core executive functions of working memory, response inhibition, and mental flexibility which are foundational processes for sustained attention, planning, and problem solving [37]. Clinically, disruption of the DLPFC circuit results in susceptibility to distraction, poor multi-tasking, organizational difficulties, and concrete or rigid thinking [45]. On neuropsychological examination, patients with lesions to the DLPFC often demonstrate difficulty with tasks such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (requiring mental flexibility and self-monitoring) and the Stroop Color Word Interference Task or other “Go/No-go” tests requiring sustained attention and response inhibition [41]. Poor performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Stroop Task, and other tests of executive functioning are commonly observed in older adults with major depression. Apathy may at least partially explain the association between executive dysfunction and depression in older adults. Funes et al. [46] found a significant association between depression severity and performance on the Stroop and Trail Making Test in LLD patients, but the strength of this association was no longer significant after apathy was entered into the statistical model. Several lines of evidence suggest that dopaminergic dysfunction contributes to the connection between apathy and cognitive impairment in LLD although direct evidence is lacking. First, animal models reveal depletion of dopamine in the DLPFC and medial prefrontal cortex results in impairments in attention and working memory and this deficit can be subsequently reversed with dopamine agonists [47]. Preliminary evidence with human subjects supports this finding. Rutherford et al. found a dose-dependent increase of L-DOPA on processing speed tasks in 36 older adults with depression. Finally, dopamine (D2/3) receptor availability in the nucleus accumbens is negatively correlated with apathy measured with the AES in depressed adults [48].

The DMPFC circuit originates in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), a prominent hub of both cognitive and reward/emotional processing. The ACC influences behavior through the initiation and monitoring of action – guiding attention in response to changes in motivation and incentive [41]. Structural changes to the ACC are therefore associated with depression and apathy [16, 49], and at the most extreme, lesions in this region result in akinetic mutism where patients have intact consciousness but are seemingly indifferent to sensory stimulation, thirst, or hunger [41]. On testing, lesions to the ACC result in significant deficits in cognitive tasks that require initiation and generation (e.g., fluency tasks) [50]. From the ACC, the DMPFC circuit projects to the ventral striatum, which includes the ventromedial caudate, ventral putamen, as well as key structures involved in reward (nucleus accumbens) and olfaction (olfactory tubercle). These connections may explain why reward systems are often found to be disrupted in apathetic patients (see more below) and why apathy is correlated with olfactory dysfunction [51].

The vmPFC circuit originates in the lateral orbital gyrus of Brodmann’s area 11 and the medial inferior frontal gyrus of the areas 10 and 47 [41]. Damage to the VMPFC results in difficulty integrating the motivational, emotional, and social aspects of behavior into decision-making. Clinically, disruption of the VMPFC results in both emotional blunting and mood lability, poor judgement, as well as lack of social tact. Profound apathy has also been reported following rare bilateral lesions to basal ganglia-VMPFC circuitry with a reduction of apathy following treatment with dopamine agonists [52]. On cognitive exam, perseverative errors – or the inability to disengage from a previous pattern of responding despite a change in stimulus – have been found to be associated with decreased volume in the VMPFC in older adults with depression [53].

VMPFC circuitry is also engaged by effort or reward-based decision-making tasks – such as the Effort Expenditure for Rewards Task (EEfRT) and Iowa Gambling Task (IGT). During the IGT participants choose cards from one of four decks with the goal to win as much money as possible [54]. Selections from two of the IGT decks are associated with higher immediate reward yet greater long-term loss, whereas the other two decks have lower immediate but better long-term gain. In order to achieve success, participants must use gain/loss feedback to guide decision-making and identify the advantageous decks. The IGT has also been used with older adults with major depression. McGovern and colleagues found older adults with depression and apathy performed in an advantageous manner on the IGT and selected more cards from the conservative decks compared with non-apathetic depressed older adults [55]. In this instance, IGT performance appears to capture a failure to become incentivized or respond to rewards in older apathetic depressed adults.

Apathy is independent from executive functioning

Apathy and executive dysfunction commonly travel together in older adults. For example, an early study by McPherson et al. found that AD patients with apathy performed significantly worse compared with AD patients without apathy on several measures of executive functioning including response inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and sustained attention [56]. Yet, it is also worth noting that several studies have found performance on tests of executive function to be independent of apathy. Marin and colleagues found no association between behavioral markers of apathy and executive functioning tests (including verbal fluency) and tests of global cognition in 52 older adults with major depression and cognitive impairment [57]. Brodaty noted that change scores in apathy were not related to subsequent change scores in executive functioning [58]. Thus, in some cases, the presence of apathy may be solely related to abnormalities in the DMPFC and not associated with classic executive functioning tests representative of DLPFC functioning [59].

Association between apathy and dementia risk in MCI

Increases in the prevalence of apathy coincide with increases in dementia severity and frontostriatal degeneration in neurodegenerative diseases such as AD, Parkinson’s disease, FTD and Huntington’s disease. For example, the estimated prevalence of apathy is 18% in MCI or very mild AD patients and 39 and 48% in mild and moderate AD patients [60] (see van Dyck et al. for an excellent review of the neurobiology of apathy in AD [61]). The presence of apathy has been therefore suggested as an early behavioral marker of impending dementia. An early study of 131 patients with amnestic MCI followed upwards of four years found that patients with both amnestic-MCI and a clinical diagnosis of apathy at baseline had an almost sevenfold risk of progression to AD compared to amnestic-MCI patients without apathy after controlling for age, sex, education, baseline global cognitive and functional status, and depression [62]. Subsequent larger studies have revealed similar findings. Using a sample of 1,821 participants with amnestic and non-amnestic MCI from the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) database, Rosenberg et al. found that the presence of apathy, irritability, and elation were the only neuropsychiatric symptoms independently associated with a subsequent transition to AD after controlling for demographics and baseline cognitive and functional status [63]. Likewise, a subsequent study also using NACC data compared the risk of developing AD in MCI patients without neuropsychiatric symptoms to MCI patients with apathy, depression, or comorbid apathy and depression [64]. Of note, MCI patients with both apathy and depression had the greatest risk of developing AD. Those with apathy only also had an elevated risk, but not those with depression only.

Association between apathy and risk of cognitive decline in community-dwelling older adults

Apathy has also been linked to a risk of cognitive decline and dementia in community-dwelling older adults without clear evidence of MCI at baseline. Clarke et al. investigated the association between apathy and cognitive decline in 1,136 older adults who participated in the Baltimore Epidemiologic Catchment Area (ECA) study [65]. Efforts were made to exclude participants with significant cognitive impairment (based upon a Mini-Mental State Exam or MMSE of less than 24) and depression at baseline. Twenty-three percent of the sample endorsed the presence of apathy at baseline using self-report. Analyses revealed an interesting association between apathy, cognition, and function. While greater baseline apathy was associated with cognitive decline (defined as a three-point difference on the MMSE) at one year after controlling for demographics and follow-up depression, change in apathy from baseline to one-year was not associated with one-year cognitive change. Apathy likewise was not associated with cognitive decline in the long-term (year 13), although apathy was associated with greater functional decline at year 13, which is consistent with evidence that apathy explains considerable variance in older adults’ everyday functioning even when accounting for depression and cognition [66, 67].

Apathy: a neurobiological perspective incorporating peripheral biomarkers and neuroimaging

Peripheral biomarkers and apathy

Across a variety of neurological and neuropsychiatric conditions, there is a growing literature on peripheral biomarkers related to presence and severity of apathy. In particular, there appear to be links between apathy and inflammatory markers. Among AD patients, compared with those without apathy, those with apathy had higher levels of interleukin-6, interleukin-10, and leptin [68]. In a study combining two large national cohorts (National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center, NACC, n = 22,760 and Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative, ADNI, n = 1733), lower Aβ42 was associated with a steep increase in apathy, while higher tau was associated with a marked decrease in apathy over a five-year period [69]. Among a cohort of stroke patients, post-stroke apathy at six months was significantly associated with elevated C-Reactive Protein (CRP) concentrations [70]. In a principal component analysis (PCA) study of patients with early Parkinson’s Disease (PD), compared with healthy control subjects, the PC with elevated IL-2 and IL-6 was associated with faster progression of Non-Motor Symptoms Scale total and mood/apathy domain scores [71].

There are also negative studies in this area. In the Leiden Study of adults 85 years and older, no association at baseline was found between C-reactive protein (CRP) concentration and apathy or depression [72]. In subjects free of apathy and depression at baseline, subjects in the highest CRP-tertile at baseline had significantly more increase in depressive symptoms but not in apathy symptoms during follow-up. While the authors concluded that apathy and depression in old age may have different etiologies, it is important to note that no inference on causality can be established. In a study of persons with HIV infection ages 50 and older, the presence of apathy was not associated with any component of a panel of potential biomarkers, including tumor necrosis factor-alpha, kynurenine, tryptophan, quinolinic acid, brain-derived neurotrophic factor, glial fibrillary acidic protein, neurofilament light chain, and phosphorylated tau at position threonine 181 [73].

Neuroimaging and apathy: an overview

As indicated in the prior section, and consistent with prior reviews on the topic, apathy has largely been attributed to structural and functional changes in the frontostriatal circuitry, particularly the ACC, OFC, ventral striatum, and insula. From a network perspective then, apathy has most often been tied to dysfunction in reward and salience networks, although increasing evidence suggests an additional role for the Executive Control Network (and its primary hub, the DLPC) in the etiology of apathy [74,75,76]. However, neuroimaging research has also yielded inconsistent findings, which are thought to be attributable to a variety of methodological issues. It is thought that such discrepancies in findings may be due to differences in apathy subtypes (i.e., variability in which symptoms predominate; see multidimensional model below), the severity of comorbid cognitive impairment, type of neuroimaging modality used in the study, imaging outcome measures, and other confounding factors (e.g., gender, race/ethnicity, education, and comorbid personality traits). These challenges result in difficulties in the validation of neuroimaging markers and have limited the clinical application of neuroimaging in the assessment of apathy. Below, we expand upon recent reviews of apathy and neuroimaging by focusing on multi-modal studies as well as investigations into apathy sub-constructs. In Table 3, we list relevant studies of apathy in AD by imaging modality. We also note similarities and differences in the neural substrates of apathy among neurodegenerative disorders and late-life depression and end with a discussion on the correlation between apathy and ischemic changes. Fortunately, with the advancement of artificial intelligence and other data analyses techniques, as well as the rapid growth of large imaging database worldwide, it is believed that neuroimaging will not only be utilized to provide information on neural mechanisms, but also can become a clinical tool assisting differential diagnoses, monitoring clinical progression and treatment effects, and development of new therapeutic targets.

Table 3 Neuroimaging studies of apathy in Alzheimer’s disease (AD).

Multi-modality neuroimaging in apathetic AD

Neuroimaging studies on the neural correlates of apathy in AD have covered a wide range of neuroimaging techniques including T1 structural magnetic resonance imaging (sMRI) for brain structural volume and/or cortical thickness, T2 fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) for white matter lesions/hyperintensities (WML/WMH), diffusion tensor imaging (DTI, or dMRI) for measures of fractional anisotropy (FA), mean diffusivity (MD) or tractography, fluorodeoxyglucose-positron emission tomography (FDG-PET) for metabolism or perfusion, Pittsburgh compound B (PiB)-PET for Aβ deposition, (11)C-PBB3-PET for tau imaging, SPECT for blood flow and resting-state fMRI (rsfMRI) and task-related fMRI (tfMRI). As demonstrated in Table 3, converging evidence suggests a disruption of the frontal-striatal circuit in apathetic AD patients. Across these studies, apathy in AD showed a significant correlation with hypoperfusion of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), putamen, and posterior cingulate (PCC) in SPECT and FDG-PET studies; cortical thinning and lower volume in these regions in sMRI studies; and lower FA in the corpus collosum, longitudinal fasciculus, and uncinate fasciculus in dMRI studies. Apathy was also correlated with greater Aβ depositions in the ACC, PFC, and putamen [77, 78] but not in one study [79], and with high tau accumulation in the OFC [79].

In addition to 11C-PBB3 PET scans for tau deposits, Kitamura and colleagues [79] also conducted PE 11C-PiB PET scans to examine Aβ deposition in AD patients with high (n = 10) and low (n = 7) Apathy Scale scores. They also acquired T1-weighted imaging to measure regional cortical thickness and diffusion tensor imaging to measure FA for white matter integrity. They investigated the association among focal Aβ and tau deposits, neural loss of focal brain, disruption of connected fibers and the severity of apathy. While they did not find significant differences in Aβ deposition between AD patients with high vs. low apathy scores, they did find an association between apathy and elevated tau deposition in OFC, decreased OFC cortical thickness, and decreased fractional anisotropy (FA) in the uncinate fasciculus, which connects to the OFC. Further path analysis indicated that the increased tau accumulation in OFC could affect apathy directly and through the reduction of OFC thickness and subsequent decrease of FA in uncinate fasciculus. This is one of the very few studies that has taken advantage of multi-modality neuroimaging data and pursued the causal relationship among findings from multi-modality imaging data. However, studies on PET often suffer from a small sample size. Even without PET imaging to confirm the pathology, more studies using multi-modality neuroimaging are still needed to better understand the disrupted brain regions or neural networks related to apathy.

A multidimensional model of apathy and associated neural circuits

Apathy is not a unitary construct. Levy and Dubois proposed three subtypes: emotional-affective, cognitive, and auto-activation apathy [80] to form a multidimensional model (see Fig. 1). Emotional apathy (also known as emotional-affective apathy), which is characterized by emotional blunting and reduced empathy, is related to the disruption of the orbito-medial stream (OFC, vmPFC, or ventral striatum). Executive apathy (also known as cognitive apathy) is characterized by difficulties in planning and organizing goals for the future and is related to the disruption of the dorsolateral stream (dlPFC, vlPFC, frontal pole, and dorsal caudate). Initiation apathy (also known as auto-activation apathy), characterized by indolence or lack of initiation, is related to deficits in the basal ganglia, the dmPFC, the premotor medial PFC, and ventral ACC. Inconsistency in these reported imaging results may be due in part to variability of different apathy subdomains across studies. The majority of studies on apathy used the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI), which does not have multidimensional constructs for studying the subdomain-associated neural mechanisms.

Fig. 1: Multimodality model of apathy (following Levy and Dubois [76]).
figure 1

Brain regions of the same color contribute to the same subtype of apathy. The dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and the right anterior insula (AI) comprise the salience network (SN), which monitors switches between internal and external attention and facilitates motivation and decision-making. The dACC connects to the ventral, dorsal, and medial-motor networks, and dysfunction of the dACC can contribute to each subtype of apathy.

Only a few studies examined subdomain-related neural substrates [81, 82]. Benoit and colleagues used the Apathy Inventory (AI) to measure the three types of apathy and found that “emotional blunting” was correlated with hypoperfusion in left superior dlPFC, “lack of interest” was correlated with hypoperfusion in right middle OFC, and “lack of initiative” was correlated with hypoperfusion in right ACC. The results do not support the hypotheses of Levy and Dubois. Using the three subdomain scores, Robert and colleagues subgrouped AD patients into “with lack of initiative and lack of interest” (n = 19) and “without lack of initiative and interest” (n = 12) [82]. When comparing AD patients with and without “lack of initiative and interest” and controlling for “emotional blunting” and NPI depression scores, the subgroup with lack of initiative and interest showed significantly lower perfusion in the right ACC than the subgroup without lack of initiative and interest. Therefore, ACC hypoperfusion is associated with “lack of initiative”, “lack of initiative and interest” as well as the total apathy score. This is not surprising given that ACC is considered a dorsal nexus [83, 84] that connects to several neural networks and plays an important role in both cognitive and emotional processing. The ACC, together with the right insula, comprise the salience network (SN), which connects with the executive control network (ECN) and default mode network (DMN) and serves as an attention switch between internally focused and externally focused mode. The ACC is also a key hub that links the valuation function of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) to the decision-making and action-taking functions of the dlPFC and supplementary motor cortex, so as to guide attention, monitor error and conflict, and modify motivation and incentive [41]. It is important to note that dysfunction of ACC is not specific to apathy; rather, it can also be found in a number of psychiatric symptoms such as depression, anxiety, and irritability.

Another SMRI study examined gray-matter intensity in AD and behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) [85]. Regardless of dementia subtype, emotional apathy was associated with loss of grey matter intensity in the cerebellum, vmPFC, and the amygdala. Executive apathy was associated with reduced integrity of the dlPFC and OFC regions. Initiation apathy was associated with lower grey matter intensity in the mPFC and ACC. These findings are concordant with the neurocircuits hypothesized in the model of multidimensional model of apathy [80].

Commonality and differences of neural substrates of apathy among neurodegenerative disorders

Apathy commonly occurs in neurodegenerative disorders such as Parkinson’s disease (PD), AD, frontotemporal dementia (FTD), and Huntington’s disease (HD). A natural area of inquiry is whether apathy of these neurodegeneration disorders involves the same neural circuits and to what extent there is overlap in brain circuitry across disorders. Based on the limited number of existing studies that have sought to address this question, the presence of apathy associated with each neurodegenerative disorder appears to be linked to more predominant neural changes. Go and colleagues compared apathy and brain atrophy between frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD, n = 30) and AD (n = 18) patients [86]. They used the Frontal System Behavior Scale (FrSBe) to assess apathy and rated bran atrophy level for the orbital (OFC), medial (mPFC), dorsolateral (dlPFC) and total prefrontal cortices (PFC) using a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0 to 4. Patients with bvFTD showed higher incidence of behavioral disturbances than AD with apathy being the most significant. BvFTD patients also demonstrated the highest incidence of atrophy in the medial and orbital frontal cortex and this atrophy was correlated with apathy. Wei and colleagues also compared apathy and brain atrophy between bvFTD and AD, using the Dimensional Apathy Scale (DAS) to quantify the emotional, executive, and initiation dimensions of apathy. They also subgrouped bvFTD and AD each into two group based on the disease duration (time since onset of first symptoms) divided into either “early” (<5 years) or “late” stages (>5 years). Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) was used to investigate differences in grey matter intensities between groups. In the early stage of the disease (< 5 years since onset), emotional apathy was present in bvFTD but not in AD. In contrast, executive apathy was more severe in the late stage (>5 years since onset) of AD compared with bvFTD. These findings can inform the development of appropriate treatment targets to ameliorate the impact of apathy in dementia, though future research is still needed to determine the extent of overlap of neural findings related to apathy across various neuropsychiatric conditions.

Comparisons of neural substrates of apathy between AD and depression

Similar to comparisons on neural substrates of apathy among neurodegenerative diseases, there are very few studies compared the similarity and differences in neural mechanisms of apathy between AD and late-life depression. Although there are no studies that directly compared apathy-related neuroimaging features between AD and late-life depression (LLD), one systematic review [87] compared findings of neuroimaging studies on apathy in LLD, brain injury, AD and other neurodegenerative disorders. The limited studies of apathy in LLD showed altered functional connectivity (FC) within the reward network and SN, with increased FC of the Nucleus Accumbens (NAcc) with dmPFC) and higher FC within the SN [88]. In contrast, Yuen and colleagues found that apathetic LLD patients had decreased (not increased) FC in the SN [16].

Apathy and depression also have overlapping components. Depression is often associated with dysfunction in the SN and reward-related network, so it follows that studies are needed that directly compare depression- and apathy-associated neural network function. A few studies directly compared the neural correlates between depression and apathy [89, 90]. Onoda et al. examined graph properties of the functional connectome in older adults with depression (n = 79), with apathy (n = 66), or with both depression and apathy (n = 33) [90]. The ACC showed lower nodal efficiency and betweenness centrality in apathy, while it showed higher nodal efficiency and betweenness centrality in depression. The results indicate that salience-related processing in the ACC is decreased in apathy and increased in depression, supporting the notion that apathy and depression are distinctive constructs.

Contribution of cerebrovascular factors to apathy in AD

Another understudied issue related to apathy in AD is whether apathy is due to AD pathology or due to microvascular structural damages and cerebrovascular disease. Studies using T2 FLAIR to examine white matter lesions (WML) or white matter hyperintensities (WMH) have partly addressed this question [91,92,93,94]. One large study (n = 651) found that focal grey matter atrophy and WMH burden were associated with worsening neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS) including apathy over time in MCI and AD, with WMH having the greater contribution to NPS [92]. Increased number of vascular risk factors worsened affective NPS. This and other studies examining WMH [87,88,89,90] are generally in agreement that worsening cerebrovascular disease contributes to the presence of apathy in AD.

Other methodological issues in current neuroimaging studies in apathy

Many neuroimaging studies have used small sample sizes and did not control for general levels of disease severity. This holds true particularly for PET and SPECT studies but was also the case for early sMRI studies as well. In these small-sample studies, one should be concerned about embracing any notions of causality. In addition, prespecified region-of-interest (ROI) analyses often predominate rather than atheoretical whole-brain studies. One caution with such “hypothesis-driven” approaches is creation of a bias in the literature that could lead to the perception of a well-established consistent association between apathy and one or more specific regions. More multi-modality studies using state-of-art data analyses methods will reduce this hypotheses-driven induced bias in the literature.

Conclusions and future directions

Apathy is a common and complex condition that clinically occurs alone but more frequently in the context of a variety of neuropsychiatric disorders. It is often found accompanied by executive dysfunction. While there are no FDA-approved pharmacological treatments for apathy, there is a mixed literature with some support for use of methylphenidate, SSRIs and cholinesterase inhibitors for apathy when it occurs in certain conditions. Repetitive TMS may hold promise as a treatment. The underlying pathophysiology is linked to pro-inflammatory processes that affect key regions of the brain, especially the frontostriatal circuits. Neuroimaging studies have helped identify possible biomarkers of apathy, but inconsistent findings may result from heterogeneity of expression of apathy (i.e., variability in which symptoms predominate), presence of comorbid neuropsychiatric disorders in which apathy is common, a spectrum of severity of cognitive function, and a variety of sociodemographic factors.

Future research should focus on identifying underlying processes and brain circuitry related to apathy across a variety of conditions, especially in depression and Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias. Such studies will help elucidate the heterogeneity of apathy and point to more tailored treatments. As potential candidate interventions are identified, subsequent clinical trials will need to account for confounding factors such as mood and cognition, and the presence of psychomotor slowing or parkinsonism [95].