Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

Test of the ballistic hypothesis of movement and the ‘point of no return’

Abstract

How long does it take to stop or modify a movement once it has begun? Where there is an error in producing an action as intended (usually because of control-display incompatibility) subjects may correct it in less than their normal reaction time (RT) by proprioception1 or efference-copy monitoring2—4. Where subjects work from a visual (external) error signal, however, they are usually said to take a full RT to stop5 or correct6,7 ongoing movements, so that there is a ‘point of no return’, after which they are committed for about 300 ms to any action decided on8. During this time the action is ballistic at the perceptual-motor level6,9,10. But in a recent tracking experiment11 where subjects wrongly anticipated the target motion, and so began a movement in the wrong direction, the correction time to stop the action (CorT) was often much shorter than the normal RT to initiate movements, down to as little as 120 ms in some cases. This suggests that fast error correction can occur from a visual signal alone, at least to stop movements (as in an emergency). Repeating the experiment we find that movements can indeed be arrested soon after they begin, but only if the beginning is delayed; it always takes one RT from the occurrence of the error signal. We therefore infer that there is a ‘point of no return’ at the decision level, but that movements are not necessarily ballistic at the motor level.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Gibbs, C. E. Br. J. Psychol. 56, 233–242 (1965).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Angel, R. W. Neurology (Minneap.) 26, 1164–1168 (1976).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Megaw, E. D. Ergonomics 15, 633–643 (1972).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Rabbitt, P. M. A. Nature 212, 438 (1966).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Hick, W. E. Q. Jl exp. Psychol. 1, 175–179 (1949).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Vince, M. Br. J. Psychol. 38, 149–157 (1948).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Craik, K. J. W. Br. J. Psychol. 38, 56–61 (1948).

    Google Scholar 

  8. Bartlett, F. C. Thinking, Chap. 1 (Allen and Unwin, London, 1958).

    Google Scholar 

  9. Searle, L. V. & Taylor, F. V. J. exp. Psychol. 38, 615–631 (1948).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Flowers, K. A. Brain 99, 269–310 (1976).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Flowers, K. A. Ergonomics (in the press).

  12. Klemmer, E. T. J. exp. Psychol. 54, 195–200 (1957).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Welford, A. T. Fundamentals of Skill, 143 (Methuen, London, 1968).

    Google Scholar 

  14. Keele, S. W. & Posner, M. I. J. exp. Psychol. 77, 155–158 (1968).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

FLOWERS, K. Test of the ballistic hypothesis of movement and the ‘point of no return’. Nature 268, 174–175 (1977). https://doi.org/10.1038/268174a0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/268174a0

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing