Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

Orientation-sensitive After-effects of Dichoptically Presented Colour and Form

Abstract

MCCOLLOUGH1 discovered that if a grating pattern of (say) red vertical stripes is viewed alternately with one of green horizontal stripes for some minutes, an orientation-specific colour after-effect is observed when viewing a black-and-white test grating. White stripes in a given orientation appear tinted with the hue complementary to the hue presented at that orientation in the original stimulus. McCollough observed that if only one eye was exposed to the original sequence of stimuli, no after-effect was seen when the other eye was used to view the test grating. From this she concluded that the adaptation responsible for the after-effect must occur somewhere in the uniocular pathway before signals from left and right eyes are combined. She suggested as a possible explanation that orientation-sensitive channels in the uniocular nervous system (“edge-detectors”) may be colour-coded, so that strong adaptation with bars of red light in one orientation leads to a “minus-red” after-response from the adapted channels to similarly oriented colourless bars. Harris and Gibson2 have confirmed that the effect persists in conditions ruling out ordinary negative afterimages, which Murch and Hirsch3 have shown sufficient to induce it. Held and Shattuck4 have demonstrated a corresponding after-effect of colour upon the perceived orientation of test bars.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. McCollough, C., Science, 149, 1115 (1965).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Harris, C. S., and Gibson, A. R., Science, 162, 1506 (1968).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Murch, G. M., and Hirsch, Joy+nm>, Amer. J. Psychol., 85, 241 (1972).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Held, R., and Shattuck, S. R., Science, 174, 314 (1971).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Creed, R. S., J. Physiol., 127, 381 (1935).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Suzuki, H., and Takahashi, M., Brain Research, 23, 161 (1970).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Sanderson, K. J., Bishop, P. O., and Darian-Smith, I., Exp. Brain Res., 13, 178 (1971).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Noda, H., Tamaki, Y., and Iwama, K., Brain Research, 41, 81 (1972).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

MACKAY, D., MACKAY, V. Orientation-sensitive After-effects of Dichoptically Presented Colour and Form. Nature 242, 477–479 (1973). https://doi.org/10.1038/242477a0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/242477a0

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing