Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

Limiting Factors in Threshold and Suprathreshold Vision

Abstract

THE threshold contrast of object detail decreases with increasing size; the full curve of Fig. 1 shows the threshold contrast of circular object details as a function of their angular diameter1. To explain the form of this function some2–4 have proposed that noise is less effective in “obscuring” the larger details; others5,6 have suggested that the contrast loss in the optical and neural processes is less for the larger details. This fundamental uncertainty has not yet been resolved7. According to the first view, noise is sampled over the equivalent area of the object detail. Because a larger number (N) of random events would be integrated in a larger detail area, the relative standard deviation (N−½) would be less, and would in fact be inversely proportional to the detail diameter (Fig. 1, dotted line). According to the second view, the “obscuring” effect of the noise is constant for different diameters of object detail, and the form of the contrast threshold function results solely from variation in the contrast loss. The loss arises from imperfections in the optical and the neural processes. The contrast loss may be quantitatively described in terms of a contrast transfer function. For the present purpose, this is defined as the ratio of the subjective brightness contrast to the objective luminance contrast of the object detail, expressed as a function of detail diameter. Both contrasts refer to the centre of the detail in relation to its background.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Blackwell, H. R., J. Opt. Soc. Amer., 36, 624 (1946).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. de Vries, Hl., Physica, 11, 179 (1943).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Rose, A., Proc. Inst. Radio Eng., 30, 295 (1942).

    Google Scholar 

  4. Rose, A., J. Opt. Soc. Amer., 38, 196 (1948).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Morgan, R. H., Amer. J. Roentgetnol. Radium Ther. Nucl. Med., 93, 982 (1965).

    Google Scholar 

  6. Dupy, O., Vision Res., 8, 1507 (1968).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Hay, G. A., Nature, 211, 1380 (1966).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Ratliff, F., Mach Bands (Holden-Day, San Francisco, 1965).

    Google Scholar 

  9. Gubisch, R. W., J. Opt. Soc. Amer., 57, 407 (1967).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  10. Campbell, F. W., and Gubisch, R. W., J. Physiol., 186, 558 (1966).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

HAY, G., CHESTERS, M. Limiting Factors in Threshold and Suprathreshold Vision. Nature 228, 1216–1218 (1970). https://doi.org/10.1038/2281216a0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/2281216a0

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing